Hippocampal neurons fire spikes when an animal is at a particular location or performs certain behaviors in a particular place, providing a cellular basis for hippocampal involvement in spatial learning and memory. In a natural environment, spatial memory is often associated with potentially dangerous sensory experiences such as noxious or painful stimuli. The central sites for such pain-associated memory or plasticity have not been identified. Here we present evidence that excitatory glutamatergic synapses within the CA1 region of the hippocampus may play a role in storing pain-related information. Peripheral noxious stimulation induced excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) in CA1 pyramidal cells in anesthetized animals. Tissue/nerve injury caused a rapid increase in the level of the immediate-early gene product Egr1 (also called NGFI-A, Krox24, or zif/268) in hippocampal CA1 neurons. In parallel, synaptic potentiation induced by a single tetanic stimulation (100 Hz for 1 s) was enhanced after the injury. This enhancement of synaptic potentiation was absent in mice lacking Egr1. Our data suggest that Egr1 may act as an important regulator of pain-related synaptic plasticity within the hippocampus.

The hippocampus and related structures are important for certain types of learning and memory in both rodents and humans (Squire and Zola-Morgan 1991; Squire 1992; Eichenbaum 1999). One special feature of hippocampal neurons is that they can fire spikes when an animal is at a particular location or performs certain behaviors in a particular location, providing a cellular basis for hippocampal involvement in spatial learning and memory (O'Keefe and Nadel 1978; Muller et al. 1987; Nadel 1991; Stevens 1996; Eichenbaum et al. 1999). In addition to spatial information, hippocampal neurons have been reported to be responsive to different sensory stimuli including noxious somatosensory stimulation (Berger et al. 1976, Berger et al. 1980, Berger et al. 1983; Brankack and Buzsaki 1986; Jirsa et al. 1992; Luntz-Leybman et al. 1992; Tamura et al. 1992; Sakurai 1994; Stevens 1996; Weiss et al. 1996). We are interested in the properties of hippocampal neuronal responses to somatosensory stimuli, especially high intensity stimuli triggering escape behavior in animals and pain in humans (Brankack and Buzsaki 1986; Sinclair and Lo 1986; Heale and Vanderwolf 1994). One possible physiological function of these responses is that the hippocampus may be involved in the formation of spatial memory associated specifically with potentially dangerous sensory experiences such as noxious or painful stimulation.

Excitatory synaptic transmission within the CA1 region of the hippocampus is mediated by glutamate. Glutamatergic synapses exhibit divergent synaptic plasticity, including long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD), depending on synaptic activity as well as postsynaptic membrane excitability (Bliss and Colingridge, 1993; Bear and Malenka 1994; Malenka and Nicoll 1999). High-frequency stimulation of excitatory synapses within the hippocampus activates various types of immediate-early genes (IEGs) in postsynaptic neurons (Morgan and Curran 1991; Bliss and Collingridge 1993; Ginty et al. 1993; Kandel 1997), including Egr1 (also called NGFI-A, Krox24, or zif/268). The IEG Egr1 is critical for coupling extracellular signals to changes in cellular gene expression (Milbrandt 1987; Deisseroth et al. 1996). In the hippocampus, Egr1 is upregulated by tetanic stimulation, which is known to induce LTP (Cole et al. 1989; Gashler and Sukhatme 1995). The function of Egr1, however, is not fully understood.

In this study, we tested the hypothesis that activity-dependent expression of Egr1 in hippocampal neurons may contribute to plastic changes in excitatory synaptic transmission. Electrophysiological, immunocytochemical, and genetic approaches were used to test this hypothesis. First, we recorded from CA1 pyramidal neurons in anesthetized adult rats and showed that some of these neurons responded to peripheral noxious stimulation. Second, using immunocytochemical staining, the IEG Egr1 was activated in hippocampal CA1 neurons after tissue injury in both rats and mice. Third, we demonstrated that in CA1 neurons, synaptic plasticity of excitatory glutamatergic transmission was altered after tissue injury. Finally, using mice lacking Egr1, we found that Egr1 was required for plastic changes in the hippocampus caused by tissue injury, in addition to contributing to long-lasting synaptic enhancement in the normal hippocampus.

Animals and Treatment

Adult male rats (Sprague-Dawley rats, 220–400 g; Harlan) and mice (wild-type and mutant Egr1 mice generated by Dr. J. Milbrandt) were used. As reported previously, we used two different amputation procedures under halothane anesthesia: in adult rats, the central digit of a rat hindpaw was removed (Wei et al. 1999); in mice, the tip of a mouse tail was removed (Zhuo 1998). We did not perform rat tail amputation in part due to surgical complications (e.g., excessive bleeding). Wild-type and homozygous mutant Egr1 mice were obtained by crossing heterozygous mutant mice bearing a targeted mutation of the Egr1 gene. Genotypes were determined by PCR analysis (Lee et al. 1995) of genomic DNA extracted from mouse ear tissue. Mice were maintained in a C57BL/6 strain background and were age matched in each experiment.

In Vivo Electrophysiology

Intracellular recording and injection of dye were performed on adult rats under halothane anesthesia. A bipolar stimulating electrode was placed into one hindpaw. Recording electrodes had a tip resistance of 50–70 MΩ when filled with a solution of 4% neurobiotin (Vector) in 2 M potassium acetate. After placement of a microelectrode in the cortex above the hippocampus (AP 3.0–6.5 mm, ML 1.0–4.0 mm), the exposed surface of the brain was covered with soft paraffin wax. After impalement, neurons with stable membrane potentials of −60 mV or greater were selected for further study. After each successful recording, neurobiotin was iontophoresed into the cell by passing a positive current pulse (2 Hz, 300 ms, 0.5–1 nA) for 10 min. At the end of the experiment, the rat was deeply anesthetized and perfused transcardially with 0.01 M phosphate-buffered saline followed by 4% paraformaldehyde. The brain was removed and stored in fixative overnight. Coronal sections were cut at a 50-μm thickness using a vibratome and incubated in 0.1% horseradish peroxidase–conjugated avidin-D (Vector) in 0.01 M potassium phosphate-buffered saline (KPBS, pH 7.4) with 0.5% Triton X-100 at room temperature for 6–8 h. After detection of peroxidase activity with 3′,3′-DAB, sections were examined in KBPS. Sections containing labeled neurons were mounted on gelatin-coated slides for light microscopy.

In Vitro Electrophysiology

Mice were anesthetized with halothane and the tail tip (2.5 cm) was removed (Zhuo 1998). Animals recovered from anesthesia within 2–3 min. In the sham-operated group, the same procedure was performed without tail amputation. Both amputated and sham-operated mice were put back in their cages with the access to food and water. 45 min later, mice were anesthetized with halothane and transverse slices of hippocampus 400 μm thick were prepared rapidly and maintained in an interface chamber at 28°C, where they were subfused with saline (artificial cerebrospinal fluid; ACSF) consisting of 124 mM NaCl, 4.4 mM CaCl2, 2.0 mM MgSO4, 25 mM NaHCO3, 1.0 mM Na2HPO4, and 10 mM glucose and bubbled with 95% O2 and 5% CO2. In some experiments, slices were harvested at different periods of time after amputation (∼0, 20, and 120 min). In a second group of experiments, mice were maintained deeply anesthetized by halothane (2–3%) for the 45 min between amputation and preparation of hippocampal slices.

In all experiments, slices recovered in the chamber for at least 2 h before recording. A bipolar tungsten stimulating electrode was placed in the stratum radiatum of the CA1 region, and extracellular field potentials were recorded using a glass microelectrode (3–12 MΩ, filled with ACSF), also in the stratum radiatum. Test responses were elicited at 0.02 Hz. In some experiments, picrotoxin (100 μM) was included in bath solution to block inhibitory transmission. LTD was induced by low frequency stimulation (1 Hz for 15 min; Dudek and Bear 1992). LTP was induced by a single tetanic stimulus (100 Hz for 1 s). Paired-pulse facilitation using various interpulse intervals (25–400 ms) was also measured. In experiments with wild-type and mutant mice, NMDA receptor-mediated field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) were measured in the presence of AMPA/kainate receptor antagonist CNQX (10 μM). Stimulation at different intensities was tested and the fiber volley-EPSP slope curves were generated. Two-pathway experiments were performed in some cases to exclude the possibility that synaptic potentiation caused after amputation may be due to nonselective gradual increase in baseline responses. Independence of the two pathways was tested by paired-pulse facilitation (see Zhuo et al. 1993). While one train tetanic stimulation was delivered to the first pathway, the second, independent pathway received no training and served as a control. One-way analysis of variance (with Duncan's multiple range test for post hoc comparison) and Student's t test were used for statistical analysis.

Immunocytochemistry

After different treatments, rats or mice were deeply anesthetized with halothane and perfused transcardially with 50–100 ml saline followed by 150–500 ml of cold 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB) containing 4% paraformaldehyde. The brain block, including hippocampus and lower lumbar and sacral spinal cord were removed, post-fixed for 4 h, and then cryoprotected by storing in a 30% sucrose, 0.1 M PB solution for 2 d at 4°C. Coronal brain and spinal cord sections (30-μm thickness) were cut using a cryostat. Sections from sham-operated and experimental animals were simultaneously processed for immunostaining. Primary rabbit antibodies used included anti-Egr1 (A310, 1:5,000; Day et al. 1990) and anti–c-Fos (1:20,000; Oncogene). Incubation with biotinylated goat anti–rabbit immunoglobulin (1:400, Vector) for 1 h was followed by incubation with avidin-biotin-peroxidase complexes (1:100, Vector) for 1 h. DAB with nickel was used as the final chromagen. Double-label immunostaining was done with anti-Egr1 and combination of a monoclonal mouse anti-CaMKIIα antiserum (1:1,000, Oncogene). Secondary antibodies conjugated to fluorescent markers FITC (1:50, used with Egr1) and Cy-3 (1:600, used with CaMKIIα; Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories) were used. Images of the CA1 areas of hippocampus sections at 0.7-μm intervals with 20× lens were obtained with Bio-Rad Laboratories MRC 1000 laser-scanning confocal fluorescent imaging system.

Immunoprecipitation of Egr1

For two mice under brief anesthesia with halothane, amputation of 2.5-cm long tail segments was performed. Two control mice received only the same brief anesthesia. After 1 h, mice were killed and hippocampi were rapidly dissected and extracted in 1.2 ml ice-cold lysis buffer (10 mM Tris, pH 7.5, 10 mM EDTA, 150 mM NaCl, 1% Triton X-100, 1% deoxycholate, 0.1% SDS, 1 mM phenylmethylsulfonyl fluoride, 1 mM leupeptin, and 1 mM pepstatin). Samples were sonicated three times and extracts were centrifuged (10,000 g for 15 min) to remove insoluble materials. Supernatants were incubated with the Egr1-specific monoclonal antibody 6H10 (Day et al. 1990) and protein A–Sepharose at 4°C overnight. The immunoprecipitates were washed three times with lysis buffer, separated on a SDS–polyacrylamide gel, transferred to nitrocellulose, and immunoblotted with polyclonal anti-Egr1 antisera (A310). This experiment was repeated twice with similar results.

Hippocampal Pyramidal Cells Respond to Peripheral Noxious Stimuli in Adult Rats In Vivo

Previous studies using extracellular field recording or spike-recording techniques revealed that neurons in the hippocampus show spike responses or mixed field potentials to peripheral noxious stimuli (Brankack and Buzsaki 1986; Heale and Vanderwolf 1994; Sinclair and Lo 1986). However, due to technical limitations, several questions remain to be addressed: (a) it is unknown whether neuronal responses originate from CA1 pyramidal neurons or local inhibitory interneurons; and (b) spike recordings fail to reveal any subthreshold (below action potential firing threshold) EPSPs. Thus, it remains unclear whether peripheral noxious stimuli could induce EPSPs in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Although subthreshold EPSPs may not elicit action potentials, they could significantly affect the electrophysiological properties as well as plasticity of hippocampal neurons.

Intracellular recordings were performed from identified hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons in anesthetized adult rats (n = 30; Fig. 1 A). All neurons were identified with neurobiotin staining as CA1 pyramidal cells (Fig. 1 C). In >35% of recorded neurons (11/30), peripheral electrical stimulation of one hindpaw elicited EPSPs. The EPSPs were intensity-related and polysynaptic in nature (Fig. 1B and Fig. D). These results provide the first direct evidence that hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons receive sensory, including nociceptive, inputs from the periphery.

Amputation Causes Rapid Expression of Egr1 in Hippocampus

Repetitive activation of excitatory glutamatergic synapses within the hippocampus activates various types of IEGs in postsynaptic neurons (Morgan and Curran 1991; Bliss and Collingridge 1993; Ginty et al. 1993; Deisseroth et al. 1996). In the hippocampus, Egr1 is upregulated by strong synaptic activity, such as tetanic stimulation, which is known to induce LTP (Cole et al. 1989; Wisden et al. 1990). Because peripheral noxious shocks elicited EPSPs in hippocampal CA1 neurons, it is conceivable that prolonged noxious stimuli may activate Egr1. We tested whether tissue/nerve injury activated Egr1 expression in hippocampal CA1 neurons. The central digit of a hindpaw in adult rats was removed. At 15 min after amputation, an increased level of Egr1 protein was detected in the CA1 region of the hippocampus (n = 5, data not shown). The level of Egr1 peaked at 45 min after amputation and remained high for at least 2 h (n = 4–6). This change in Egr1 expression was regionally selective within the CA1 region of the hippocampus, as only minor changes were detected in the CA3 region or dentate gyrus (DG).

To use genetically manipulated mice, we tested whether similar changes could be observed in mice after tissue/nerve injury. Removal of the tip of a mouse tail induced an NMDA-dependent, long-lasting hyperalgesia in mice (Zhuo 1998). Accordingly, the removal of a distal tail segment caused a significant increase of the expression of Egr1 in the hippocampus, as revealed by both immunoprecipitation and immunocytochemistry (Fig. 2A and Fig. B). Similar to our observations in rats, increased levels of Egr1 were most dramatic in the CA1 region of the hippocampus (Fig. 2 B). To better demonstrate that Egr1 activation occurred within CA1 pyramidal cells, we carried out double stainings with both Egr1 and calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII). We found strong nuclear Egr1 signal expression in CA1 pyramidal neurons visualized by CaMKII immunofluorescence (n = 2 mice, see Fig. 2 C for an example).

Only minor changes were observed in CA3 or DG (Table). Egr1 expression is selective for noxious stimuli; in experiments using non-noxious mechanical brush (with paintbrush for 12 s, n = 3) or non-noxious heating (at 40°C for 12 s, n = 4), we did not see any significant increase of Egr1 expression in the hippocampus.

To determine whether Egr1 was upregulated in neurons of the spinal dorsal horn or brainstem in response to injury, we examined its expression and that of c-Fos after amputation of the distal tail. While significant increases in c-Fos expression were observed in dorsal horn neurons, little or no change in Egr1 levels was observed in the spinal cord (Fig. 2 F and Table) or rostral ventromedial medulla (RVM; data not shown).

Peripheral Sensory Inputs and Activation of NMDA Receptors

Heightened Egr1 expression caused by amputation was NMDA receptor dependent. Pretreatment with i.p. injected MK-801 (1 mg/kg, 30 min before amputation) attenuated amputation-induced increases in Egr1 expression in the hippocampus (Fig. 2 D and Table). In mice receiving i.p. morphine (10 mg/kg; 30 min before amputation) and local anesthetic blockade with subcutaneously applied QX-314 (5%; 10 μl, 10 min before), the amputation induced significantly less Egr1 expression in the hippocampus (Fig. 2 E and Table). These findings indicate that sensory inputs during tissue/nerve injury were critical for the induction of Egr1.

Plasticity within the Hippocampus after Amputation

Could changes in Egr1 expression also be accompanied by plastic changes in excitatory transmission in the CA1 region? Two major forms of synaptic plasticity have been reported in the hippocampus: LTP and LTD (Bear and Malenka 1994). First, we measured LTP induced by a single tetanic stimulation (100 Hz, 1 s). In slices from sham-operated mice, a single tetanic stimulation induced only a small synaptic potentiation that lasted ∼1 h (n = 6, 141.2 ± 15.4% of control at 1 h after stimulation; Fig. 3 A). In contrast, a significantly greater potentiation was induced in slices prepared from mice 45 min after tail tip amputation (n = 6, 259.3 ± 40.4%, t(10) = 2.73, P < 0.05 compared with sham mice; Fig. 3 A). This potentiation lasted for at least 2–4 h (n = 3; see Fig. 3 B for an example). To demonstrate that synaptic enhancement was input specific, we also performed two-pathway experiments. While one train tetanic stimulation caused prolonged enhancement in one pathway (n = 4, 189.4 ± 3.4%), synaptic responses recorded from the second, independent pathway remained unchanged (n = 4, 91.5 ± 9.8%; Fig. 3 C). The effect of amputation on synaptic potentiation was time related. We also carried out experiments using mice killed at three different time points after amputation (∼0, 20, and 120 min after amputation). As shown in Fig. 3 D, we found no significant enhancement of synaptic potentiation induced by one train tetanic stimulation at either 0 min or 20 min after amputation. A similar amount of enhancement was observed in slices prepared at 120 min after amputation (n = 5, 270.3 ± 37.8%) relative to that at 45 min after amputation (n = 6, 258.2 ± 37.9%).

To test if the conscious experience of pain during the 45 min between the tail amputation and decapitation contribute to the observed alterations in hippocampal synaptic potentiation, we kept mice anesthetized throughout the 45 min between amputation and slice preparation in some experiments. Interestingly, we found that no significant enhancement of synaptic potentiation (n = 4, 131.5 ± 3.7%; no significant difference from slices of sham-treated animals). In addition, we measured Egr1 activation in hippocampus from mice receiving continuous halothane anesthesia after amputation and found that activation of Egr1 was completely blocked in these mice (n = 2).

LTD induced by low frequency stimulation (1 Hz, 15 min) was not affected by amputation (sham: n = 7, 66.8 ± 13.4% of control at 30 min after stimulation; amputated: n = 5, 50.3 ± 11.8%, no significant difference between the two groups; Fig. 3 E). To detect possible frequency-dependent changes, we applied the same number of pulses (n = 900) at two additional frequencies, 5 and 10 Hz. No significant difference was found between sham-operated and amputated mice (Fig. 3F and Fig. G). Basal synaptic responses were not significantly different between sham (n = 25 slices/10 mice) and amputated animals (45 min after the amputation; n = 30 slices/15 mice, data not shown). Furthermore, paired-pulse facilitation, an indication of possible presynaptic changes, was also not affected (sham: n = 9; amputated: n = 16, data not shown).

Activation of Egr1 in Hippocampal Slices

What is the molecular mechanism contributing to the enhancement of synaptic potentiation after amputation? We hypothesized that NMDA receptor-dependent Egr1 activation may play an important role in the synaptic enhancement caused by amputation. To test this, we first determined whether hippocampal slices could be used to detect possible changes in gene expression caused by amputation. Studies from different regions of the central nervous system have shown that some physiological changes can be detected using an in vitro brain slice technique (Shors et al. 1989; Kirkwood et al. 1996; Malenka and Nicoll 1997; Wei et al. 1999). In hippocampal slices incubated in a recording chamber for at least 2 h, we found that levels of Egr1 were not upregulated (n = 6, data not shown). Bath application of glutamate (100 μM) increased Egr1 in CA1 neurons (n = 7). Moreover, activation of NMDA receptors was critical for this upregulation, because pretreatment with 50 μM AP-5 blocked Egr1 activation (n = 6). Thus, activation of Egr1 in vitro was also NMDA receptor dependent, as shown in vivo.

Hippocampal Synaptic Potentiation and Depression in Mice Lacking Egr1

Second, we wanted to determine if Egr1 contributes to hippocampal LTD and LTP using mice lacking Egr1 (Lee et al. 1995). LTD induced by a prolonged, 1 Hz stimulation (for 15 min) was not affected in mutant mice (wild-type: n = 8, 60.0 ± 10.5%; mutant: n = 6, 75.2 ± 7.5%, no significant difference between two groups; Fig. 4 A). Synaptic responses to repetitive stimulation at two other frequencies (5 and 10 Hz) were also not affected (Fig. 4 B; data with 10 Hz not shown). Moreover, as shown in Fig. 4 C, no significant difference in LTP induced by a single tetanic stimulation was found between the two groups (wild-type: n = 8, 121.8 ± 10.2%; mutant: n = 5, 127.5 ± 8.2%). Next, we sought to determine whether Egr1 is important for the enhancement of LTP caused by amputation (see Fig. 3 A). In both wild-type and mutant mice, we used alternative method for genotyping (i.e., by PCR analysis of genomic DNA extracted from mouse ear tissue). We performed experiments on Egr1-deficient and wild-type mice after amputation of the distal tail. In contrast to wild-type mice (n = 5, 238.3 ± 36.1% of control at 45 min after tetanic stimulation), amputation in mutant mice failed to cause enhancement of LTP (n = 6, 125.8 ± 8.8%, t(9) = 3.02, P <0.01 compared with wild-type mice; Fig. 4D and Fig. E). This result suggests that Egr1 upregulation induced by amputation is critical for amputation-induced enhancement of LTP. Pharmacological inhibition of Egr1 activation with i.p. MK-801 (1 mg/kg, 30 min before amputation) revealed similar results (n = 4).

Egr1 Contributes to Late-Phase LTP

What could be the possible physiological functions of Egr1 in hippocampal plasticity? One possible function of Egr1 is to contribute to LTP. We performed several additional electrophysiological experiments in wild-type and mutant mice. First, we measured paired-pulse facilitation at different interpulse intervals. No significant difference was found between wild-type (n = 6 mice) and mutant mice (n = 8 mice; Fig. 5 A). Basal synaptic responses to stimulation were also not significantly different between wild-type (n = 18 slices/11 mice) and mutant animals (n = 19 slices/12 mice). However, late-phase LTP induced by a four train tetanic stimulation (39–40) was significantly decreased in mutant mice (wild-type: n = 9, 210.7 ± 21.3%; mutant: n = 5, 132.6 ± 29.0%, t(12) = 2.18, P < 0.05; Fig. 5 D). Pharmacological experiments using 100 μM AP-5 demonstrated that the NMDA receptor is essential for induction and expression of late-phase LTP (n = 4, 92.2 ± 13.2%, Fig. 5 C). To detect if NMDA receptor-mediated responses may be affected in mutant mice, we measured NMDA receptor-mediated EPSPs in the presence of the AMPA/kainate receptor antagonist CNQX (10 μM). We found no significant difference between wild-type (n = 9 slices/6 mice) and mutant animals (n = 6 slices/5 mice; Fig. 5 B), indicating that NMDA receptor function is not significantly affected. Finally, we also measured late-phase LTP of mutant slices in the presence of picrotoxin (100 μM) and found a similar defect in late-phase LTP (n = 4, 138.9 ± 8.3%). These results suggest the observed defect in late-phase LTP in mutant mice is not due to changes in NMDA receptor functions or inhibitory tone.

In this study, we show that noxious somatosensory stimuli elicit both EPSPs and elevations in Egr1 expression in hippocampal CA1 neurons. The same maneuver, tail tip amputation, that led to the latter result also led to an enhancement of synaptic potentiation in the CA1 region. Moreover, this enhancement of plasticity was not present in mice lacking Egr1. Finally, late-phase LTP but not early-phase LTP or LTD was abolished in Egr1 knockout mice.

It may not be readily evident why tail tip amputation should induce changes in hippocampal physiology different from decapitation, which is performed during hippocampal slice preparation. We hypothesized that after an extremely painful stimulus, such as tail tip amputation, a mouse, awake and alert after recovering from the brief, light anesthesia, would react in many ways, in particular, that changes may occur in susceptibility to synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus. According to this model, decapitation would induce none of these effects, the animal never experiences that stimulation consciously. Supporting this hypothesis, we found that keeping mice under halothane anesthesia between tail amputation and decapitation blocked the enhancement of synaptic potentiation. Furthermore, experiments using slices harvested at different time points after amputation consistently demonstrated that a certain period of consciousness is required for synaptic enhancement to occur. Slices were allowed to recover in the recording chamber for at least 2 h (often more time elapsed before the beginning of a given recording). Since 45 min between amputation and slice preparation was sufficient time to achieve a maximal effect on hippocampal synaptic potentiation, more than sufficient time elapsed after slice preparation to allow any similar effect of that maneuver to take hold. Taken together, these observations lend strong support to our conclusion that the difference observed between sham-operated and amputated animals was specifically a result of the physiological responses of a conscious animal during the minutes following severe tissue/nerve injury.

Electrical and Biochemical Responses of Hippocampal Neurons to Somatosensory Stimuli

We demonstrate in this study that >35% of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells respond to peripheral noxious shocks, providing the first intracellular in vivo recordings to support previous observations implicating the hippocampus in pain-related physiological functions. Previous electrophysiological evidence using extracellular recording techniques failed to determine which neurons were the source of the signal recorded. In addition, peripheral stimulation generates many subthreshold depolarizations in CA1 neurons which would not generate spikes but may nonetheless influence the summated output.

Interestingly, these EPSPs showed intensity-related responses, that is, larger EPSPs were observed with higher-intensity peripheral electrical shocks. These findings suggest that these neurons may also encode the intensity of stimulation. Gentle touch, however, did not cause any significant responses. We should point out that not all CA1 neurons responded to peripheral noxious shocks, suggesting that only a subpopulation of CA1 neurons are responsive to noxious stimuli. Hippocampal neurons receive inputs from many areas of the CNS, including the rostroventral medulla (Vertes and Kocsis 1997), suggesting that there are multiple potential pathways through which these hippocampal CA1 neurons can be activated.

Hippocampal neurons also respond to somatosensory stimuli by changes in gene expression. Mixed changes in the expression of c-Fos in the hippocampus after tissue injury has been reported previously, including increases after subcutaneous formalin injection (Aloisi et al. 1997) or decreases after noxious tooth pulp stimulation (Funahashi et al. 1999). However, c-Fos seems not to play an important role in hippocampal synaptic transmission and plasticity. For example, mice lacking c-Fos showed normally early- and late-phase LTP (unpublished observation). In this study, using a different animal model, we showed that tissue injury (amputation of a single digit in rats or distal tail in mice) causes rapid expression of Egr1 in the hippocampus, including CA1 neurons. In awake animals, activation of Egr1 requires noxious stimulation. Non-noxious stimuli, such as mechanical brush and warm thermal heating, failed to induce significant Egr1 expression in the hippocampus. Most of the expression was seen in the CA1 area but not DG of the hippocampus.

Activation of NMDA receptors was important for the expression of Egr1 caused by tissue injury in vivo or bath application of glutamate to hippocampal slices in vitro. These results are consistent with previous studies in rats showing that Egr1 expression was induced by synaptic activity through NMDA receptors (see Introduction). In the spinal cord, we found that Egr1 was not significantly activated in dorsal horn sensory neurons. These results differ from a previous report using heat-induced tissue injury (Wisden et al. 1990), suggesting that amputation may activate central pathways distinct from those activated by some other types of tissue injury (see Zhuo 1998, for discussion).

Egr1 Contributes to Hippocampal Synaptic Potentiation

Despite the well-documented, activity-dependent stimulation of Egr1 expression in hippocampal neurons, no report is available concerning a possible contribution of Egr1 to NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic plasticity within the hippocampus. In this study, using mice lacking Egr1 gene, we present the first evidence that Egr1 is important for late-phase LTP. This effect of Egr1 deletion is relatively selective. LTP induced by a single tetanic stimulation or LTD were normal in mice lacking Egr1. Furthermore, other basic electrophysiological properties of these synapses seem to be normal (e.g., paired-pulse facilitation, basal field EPSPs and NMDA receptor-mediated EPSPs). It is obviously important to identify, in future studies, further cellular target proteins downstream from Egr1 which may contribute to an enhancement of synaptic potentiation. Although Egr1 is also upregulated by seizure (Mack et al. 1990), genetic deletion of Egr1 in mice seems to have no effect on the rate of kindling and associated mossy fiber sprouting in the hippocampus (Zhang et al. 1998).

Synaptic Enhancement after Amputation

One typical question for gene-related pathways is whether they are activated under physiological/pathological conditions and, upon activation, how they may affect the properties of central synapses. This study provides a possible answer for Egr1 in the hippocampus. We found that activation of Egr1 within the hippocampus occurs after tissue injury. Parallel with the activation of Egr1 by tissue injury, changes in synaptic plasticity obtained from hippocampal slices from animals with tissue injury were observed. A single tetanic stimulation, which normally induced moderate synaptic potentiation, induced a larger and longer-lasting potentiation in the CA1 area of the hippocampus after amputation. Interestingly, we found that keeping amputated animals anesthetized throughout the 45 min between amputation and hippocampal slice preparation prevented synaptic enhancement. Consistent with electrophysiological observations, the activation of Egr1 by amputation was also blocked. Halothane is known to decrease central neuronal excitability by inhibiting excitatory glutamatergic transmission and enhancing inhibitory transmission in rats and mice (Jones et al. 1992; Jones and Harrison 1993; Perousansky et al. 1995; Kirson et al. 1998). It is likely that halothane inhibits glutamatergic transmission (such as the activation of NMDA receptors) and prevents the activation of Egr1, which is required for synaptic enhancement caused by amputation.

The other forms of synaptic plasticity tested were not affected, such as paired-pulse facilitation and LTD. These changes are not likely due to general stress during or after the amputation. Hippocampal synaptic plasticity is differentially affected in animals under behavioral stress compared with animals after tail amputation. After behavioral stress, hippocampal LTP was inhibited (Shors et al. 1989; Diamond et al. 1994) and LTD was facilitated (Xu et al. 1997), suggesting that distinct mechanisms are involved.

We believe that the synaptic enhancement after amputation did not reflect a generally elevated neuronal excitability. First, in experiments using two-pathway stimulation in the same slice, we showed that synaptic enhancement was selectively observed in the pathway receiving a single tetanic stimulation. Synaptic responses were not affected at the second pathway. Second, we have showed that basal responses to stimulation as well as paired-pulse facilitation were not affected after amputation. Finally, if neuronal excitability were uniformly enhanced, we might be expected to see less LTP (due to an occlusive, or saturating, effect); instead, we observed the opposite effect (enhanced LTP).

We also further test the possible relationship between activated Egr1 and changes in synaptic plasticity using mice lacking Egr1. Interestingly, the enhancement of potentiation caused by a single tetanic stimulation was blocked in mice lacking Egr1. These results suggest that Egr1 or Egr1-related signaling pathways could serve as a temporary marker within neurons for peripheral tissue injury.

Physiological Significance

Although the evidence we have so far does not allow us to assign confidently any physiological role to Egr1 in vivo, we suggest that Egr1 might serve as an important molecule for nociception or pain-related plasticity within the hippocampus. Egr1 may not only serve as a signaling molecule downstream from the NMDA receptor, thereby contributing to late-phase LTP, but it may also associate sensory nociceptive, non-spatial information with spatial memory. The hippocampus and related structures are known to play a critical role in spatial as well as non-spatial memory formation (Squire and Zola-Morgan 1991; Squire 1992; Eichenbaum 1999). Such an association may explain why a patient retains a vivid memory of the place where an injury was sustained. Second, the hippocampus may also affect the perception of pain or pain-related unpleasantness. Lesion of the hippocampus or reversible blockade of hippocampal neuronal activity are reported to affect behavioral nociceptive responses in animals (Jackson and Regestein 1979; Plaznik et al. 1983; McKenna and Melzack 1992) and humans (Gol and Faibisch 1966, Gol and Faibisch 1967). Our results provide evidence at molecular and cellular levels that the hippocampus may play a role in pain memory.

We want to thank all members of the Zhuo's laboratory for helpful suggestions.

This work was supported in part by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIDA 10833 and NINDS 38680 to M. Zhuo; National Cancer Institute to J. Milbrandt; and NINDS to Z.C. Xu).

Aloisi
A.M.
,
Zimmermann
M.
,
Herdegen
T.
Sex-dependent effects of formalin and restraint on c-Fos expression in the septum and hippocampus of the rat
Neuroscience
81
1997
951
958
[PubMed]
Bear
M.F.
,
Malenka
R.C.
Synaptic plasticityLTP and LTD
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol
4
1994
389
399
[PubMed]
Berger
T.W.
,
Alger
B.
,
Thompson
R.F.
Neuronal substrate of classical conditioning in the hippocampus
Science.
192
1976
483
485
[PubMed]
Berger
T.W.
,
Laham
R.I.
,
Thompson
R.F.
Hippocampal unit-behavior correlations during classical conditioning
Brain Res
193
1980
229
248
[PubMed]
Berger
T.W.
,
Rinaldi
P.C.
,
Weisz
D.J.
,
Thompson
R.F.
Single-unit analysis of different hippocampal cell types during classical conditioning of rabbit nictitating membrane response
J. Neurophysiol.
50
1983
1197
1219
[PubMed]
Bliss
T.V.
,
Collingridge
G.L.
A synaptic model of memorylong-term potentiation in the hippocampus
Nature.
361
1993
31
39
[PubMed]
Brankack
J.
,
Buzsaki
G.
Hippocampal response evoked by tooth pulp and acoustic stimulationdepth profiles and effect of behavior
Brain Res
378
1986
303
314
[PubMed]
Cole
A.J.
,
Saffen
D.W.
,
Baraban
J.M.
,
Worley
P.F.
Rapid increase of an immediate-early gene messenger RNA in hippocampal neurons by synaptic NMDA receptor activation
Nature.
340
1989
474
476
[PubMed]
Day
M.L.
,
Fahrner
T.J.
,
Aykent
S.
,
Milbrandt
J.
The zinc finger protein NGFI-A exists in both nuclear and cytoplasmic forms in nerve growth factor-stimulated PC 12 cells
J. Biol. Chem.
265
1990
15253
15260
[PubMed]
Deisseroth
K.
,
Bito
H.
,
Tsien
R.W.
Signaling from synapse to nucleuspostsynaptic CREB phosphorylation during multiple forms of hippocampal synaptic plasticity
Neuron.
16
1996
89
101
[PubMed]
Diamond
D.M.
,
Fleshner
M.
,
Rose
G.M.
Psychological stress repeatedly blocks hippocampal primed burst potentiation behaving rats
Behav. Brain Res.
62
1994
1
9
[PubMed]
Dudek
S.M.
,
Bear
M.F.
Homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of hippocampus and effects of N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor blockade
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA.
89
1992
4363
4367
[PubMed]
Eichenbaum
H.
Conscious awareness, memory and the hippocampus
Nat. Neurosci.
2
1999
775
776
[PubMed]
Eichenbaum
H.
,
Dudchenko
P.
,
Wood
E.
,
Shapiro
M.
,
Tanila
H.
The hippocampus, memory, and place cellsis it spatial memory or a memory space?
Neuron.
23
1999
209
226
[PubMed]
Funahashi
M.
,
He
Y.F.
,
Sugimoto
T.
,
Matsuo
R.
Noxious tooth pulp stimulation suppresses c-fos expression in the rat hippocampal formation
Brain Res.
827
1999
215
220
[PubMed]
Gashler
A.
,
Sukhatme
V.P.
Early growth response protein 1 (Egr1)prototype of a zinc-finger family of transcription factors
Prog. Nucleic Acid Res. Mol. Biol.
50
1995
191
224
[PubMed]
Ginty
D.D.
,
Kornhauser
J.M.
,
Thompson
M.A.
,
Bading
H.
,
Mayo
K.E.
,
Takahashi
J.S.
,
Greenberg
M.E.
Regulation of CREB phosphorylation in the suprachiasmatic nucleus by light and a circadian clock
Science.
260
1993
238
241
[PubMed]
Gol
A.
,
Faibisch
G.M.
Hippocampectomy for relief of intractable pain
Tex. Med.
62
1966
76
79
[PubMed]
Gol
A.
,
Faibisch
G.M.
Effects of human hippocampal ablation
J. Neurosurg.
26
1967
390
398
[PubMed]
Heale
V.R.
,
Vanderwolf
C.H.
Dentate gyrus and olfactory bulb responses to olfactory and noxious stimulation in urethane anaesthetized rats
Brain Res.
652
1994
235
242
[PubMed]
Jackson
W.J.
,
Regestein
Q.R.
Hippocampal lesions impair prolonged titrated avoidance by rhesus monkeys
Exp. Neurol.
63
1979
28
34
[PubMed]
Jirsa
R.
,
Poc
P.
,
Radil
T.
Hippocampal auditory evoked response threshold in the ratbehavioral modulation
Brain Res. Bull.
28
1992
149
153
[PubMed]
Jones
M.V.
,
Harrison
N.L.
Effects of volatile anesthetics on the kinetics of inhibitory postsynaptic currents in cultured rat hippocampal neurons
J. Neurophysiol.
70
1993
1339
1349
[PubMed]
Jones
M.V.
,
Brooks
P.A.
,
Harrison
N.L.
Enhancement of gamma-aminobutyric acid-activated Cl-currents in cultured rat hippocampal neurons by three volatile ananesthetics
J. Physiol.
449
1992
279
293
[PubMed]
Kandel
E.R.
Genes, synapses, and long-term memory
J. Cell. Physiol.
173
1997
124
125
[PubMed]
Kirkwood
A.
,
Rioult
M.G.
,
Bear
M.F.
Experience-dependent modification of synaptic plasticity in visual cortex
Nature.
381
1996
526
528
[PubMed]
Kirson
E.D.
,
Yaari
Y.
,
Perouansky
M.
Presynaptic and postsynaptic actions of halothane at glutamatergic synapses in the mouse hippocampus
Brit. J. Pharmacol.
124
1998
1607
1614
[PubMed]
Lee
S.L.
,
Tourtellotte
L.C.
,
Wesselschmidt
R.L.
,
Milbrandt
J.
Growth and differentiation proceeds normally in cells deficient in the immediate-early gene NGFI-A
J. Biol. Chem.
270
1995
9971
9977
[PubMed]
Luntz-Leybman
V.
,
Bickford
P.C.
,
Freedman
R.
Cholinergic gating of response to auditory stimuli in rat hippocampus
Brain Res.
587
1992
130
136
[PubMed]
Mack
K.
,
Day
M.
,
Milbrandt
J.
,
Gottlieb
D.I.
Localization of the NGFI-A protein in the rat brain
Mol. Brain Res.
8
1990
177
180
[PubMed]
Malenka
R.C.
,
Nicoll
R.A.
Learning and memorynever fear, LTP is hear
Nature.
390
1997
552
553
[PubMed]
Malenka
R.C.
,
Nicoll
R.A.
Long-term potentiation—a decade of progress?
Science.
285
1999
1870
1874
[PubMed]
McKenna
J.E.
,
Melzack
R.
Analgesia produced by lidocaine microinjection into the dentate gyrus
Pain.
49
1992
105
112
[PubMed]
Milbrandt
J.
A nerve growth factor-induced gene encodes a possible transcriptional regulatory factor
Science.
238
1987
797
798
[PubMed]
Morgan
J.L.
,
Curran
T.
Stimulus-transcription coupling in the nervous systeminvolvement of the inducible proto-oncogenes fos and jun
Annu. Rev. Neurosci.
14
1991
421
451
[PubMed]
Muller
R.U.
,
Kubie
J.L.
,
Ranck
J.B.
Jr.
Spatial firing patterns of hippocampal complex-spike cells in a fixed environment
J. Neurosci.
7
1987
1935
1950
[PubMed]
Nadel
L.
The hippocampus and space revisited
Hippocampus.
1
1991
221
229
[PubMed]
O'Keefe
J.
,
Nadel
L.
The hippocampus as a cognitive map
1978
Oxford University Press
London
Perousansky
M.
,
Baranov
D.
,
Salman
M.
,
Yaari
Y.
Effects of halothane on glutamate receptor-mediated excitatory postsynaptic currents. A patch-clamp study in adult mouse hippocampal slices
Anesthesiology.
83
1995
109
119
[PubMed]
Plaznik
A.
,
Danysz
W.
,
Kostowski
W.
Some behavioral effects of microinjections of noradrenaline and serotonin into the hippocampus of the rat
Physiol. Behav.
31
1983
625
631
[PubMed]
Sakurai
Y.
Involvement of auditory cortical and hippocampal neurons in auditory working memory and reference memory in the rat
J. Neurosci.
14
1994
2606
2623
[PubMed]
Shors
T.J.
,
Seib
T.B.
,
Levine
S.
,
Thompson
R.F.
Inescapable versus escapable shock modulates long-term potentiation in the rat hippocampus
Science.
244
1989
224
226
[PubMed]
Sinclair
J.G.
,
Lo
G.F.
Morphine, but not atropine, blocks nociceptor-driven activity in rat dorsal hippocampal neurons
Neurosci. Lett.
68
1986
47
50
[PubMed]
Squire
L.R.
Memory and the hippocampusa synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans
Psychol. Rev.
99
1992
195
231
[PubMed]
Squire
L.R.
,
Zola-Morgan
S.
The medial temporal lobe memory system
Science.
253
1991
1380
1386
[PubMed]
Stevens
C.F.
Spatial learning and memorythe beginning of a dream
Cell.
87
1996
1147
1148
[PubMed]
Tamura
R.
,
Ono
T.
,
Fukuda
M.
,
Nakamura
K.
Spatial responsiveness of monkey hippocampal neurons to various visual and auditory stimuli
Hippocampus.
2
1992
307
322
[PubMed]
Vertes
R.P.
,
Kocsis
B.
Brainstem-diencephalo-septohippocampal systems controlling the theta rhythm of the hippocampus
Neuroscience.
81
1997
893
926
[PubMed]
Wei
F.
,
Li
P.
,
Zhuo
M.
Loss of synaptic depression in mammalian anterior cingulate cortex following amputation
J. Neuroscience.
19
1999
9346
9354
Weiss
C.
,
Kronforst-Collins
M.A.
,
Disterhoft
J.F.
Activity of hippocampal pyramidal neurons during trace eyeblink conditioning
Hippocampus.
6
1996
192
209
[PubMed]
Wisden
W.
,
Errington
M.L.
,
Williams
S.
,
Dunnett
S.B.
,
Waters
C.
,
Hitchcock
D.
,
Evan
G.
,
Bliss
T.V.P.
,
Hunt
S.P.
Differential expression of immediate-early genes in the hippocampus and spinal cord
Neuron.
4
1990
603
614
[PubMed]
Xu
L.
,
Anwyl
R.
,
Rowan
M.J.
Behavioral stress facilitates the induction of long-term depression in the hippocampus
Nature.
387
1997
497
500
[PubMed]
Zhang
D.
,
Butler
L.S.
,
McNamara
J.O.
Kindling and associated mossy fibre sprouting are not affected in mice deficient of NGFI-A/NGFI-B genes
Neuroscience.
83
1998
251
258
[PubMed]
Zhuo
M.
,
Small
S.A.
,
Kandel
E.R.
,
Hawkins
R.D.
Nitric oxide and carbon monoxide produce activity-dependent long-term synaptic enhancement in hippocampus
Science.
260
1993
1946
1950
[PubMed]
Zhuo
M.
NMDA receptor-dependent long term hyperalgesia after tail amputation in mice
Eur. J. Pharmacol
349
1998
211
220
[PubMed]

Abbreviations used in this paper: ACSF, artificial cerebrospinal fluid; DG, dentate gyrus; EPSPs, excitatory postsynaptic potentials; IEGs, immediate-early genes; LTD, long-term depression; LTP, long-term potentiation.