Abnormal epigenetic regulation can cause the nervous system to develop abnormally. Here, we sought to understand the mechanism by which this occurs by investigating the protein complex cohesin, which is considered to regulate gene expression and, when defective, is associated with higher-level brain dysfunction and the developmental disorder Cornelia de Lange syndrome (CdLS). We generated conditional Smc3-knockout mice and observed greater dendritic complexity and larger numbers of immature synapses in the cerebral cortex of Smc3+/− mice. Smc3+/− mice also exhibited more anxiety-related behavior, which is a symptom of CdLS. Further, a gene ontology analysis after RNA-sequencing suggested the enrichment of immune processes, particularly the response to interferons, in the Smc3+/− mice. Indeed, fewer synapses formed in their cortical neurons, and this phenotype was rescued by STAT1 knockdown. Thus, low levels of cohesin expression in the developing brain lead to changes in gene expression that in turn lead to a specific and abnormal neuronal and behavioral phenotype.

Accumulating evidence indicates that alteration of the chromosomal architecture leads to transcriptional changes during the formation of the neuronal network (Misteli, 2007; Takizawa and Meshorer, 2008; Li et al., 2012; Kohwi and Doe, 2013; Nord et al., 2015). Abnormal epigenetic regulation of gene expression during this period causes several developmental disorders of the nervous system, such as autism spectrum disorder and Rett syndrome (Urdinguio et al., 2009; Bourgeron, 2015).

Cohesin is a highly conserved nuclear protein complex known to regulate not only higher-order chromatin organization, but also gene expression (Dorsett, 2007; Wendt et al., 2008). It is composed of four subunits (Smc1, Smc3, Scc3, and Scc1; Guacci et al., 1997; Michaelis et al., 1997; Losada et al., 1998), which form a ring structure (Nasmyth and Haering, 2005; Nasmyth, 2011). During cell division, the cohesin ring binds sister chromatids together from S phase until the subsequent anaphase, thus ensuring proper chromosomal segregation during the cell cycle.

Furthermore, cohesin is expressed not only in dividing cells but also in postmitotic cells (Wendt et al., 2008), suggesting that cohesin may also have cohesion-independent functions. In yeast, cohesin helps establish the boundaries that prevent the spread of silenced chromatin (Donze et al., 1999). In Drosophila melanogaster, Nipped-B, which is required to load cohesin onto DNA, mediates long-range activation of the homeobox genes cut and ultrabithorax (Rollins et al., 1999). Previous studies have indicated that cohesin functions in axon pruning of the Drosophila mushroom-body γ neurons. Its disruption leads to axon pruning defects in Drosophila (Pauli et al., 2008; Schuldiner et al., 2008), presumably through altered gene expression triggered by inhibition of cohesin, although direct evidence for this mechanism is lacking.

Cohesin also functions in the human central nervous system (CNS). Mutations that perturb its function, or those that affect the proteins that regulate it, cause Cornelia de Lange syndrome (CdLS; Krantz et al., 2004; Tonkin et al., 2004; Musio et al., 2006), a rare malformation syndrome characterized by intellectual disability, limb abnormalities, and distinctive facial features. Most of these mutations do not cause overt defects in cohesin itself or in chromosomal segregation, indicating that the disease phenotype does not result from defective cohesion. The developmental defects seen in humans might therefore be caused by altered gene expression due to the disruption of chromosomal architecture by malfunctioning cohesin; however, evidence for this hypothesis is lacking.

We postulated that epigenetic control of gene expression by cohesin is required for normal brain development and higher brain function. Here, we show that abnormally low cohesin expression in neurons caused abnormal dendrite and synapse formation in mice, which led to increased anxiety-related behavior in mice. Further, gene ontology (GO) analysis revealed that genes associated with CNS development were altered in the cortex of Smc3+/− mice. Thus, sufficient expression of cohesin must be present in the developing brain to ensure proper epigenetic control of several genes and a specific neuronal and behavioral phenotype. Cohesin thus acts as a global gene regulator and contributes to a phase of neuronal network formation.

Generation of conditional Smc3-knockout mice

Smc3 is the critical subunit to establish the ring structure of cohesin. We first investigated the expression of cohesin in terminally differentiated neurons by performing immunohistochemistry for Smc3 in mouse brain tissue. Brain sections from postnatal week 6 (P6w) mice were immunostained with anti-Smc3 and anti-NeuN. Smc3 was expressed ubiquitously in the brain in both NeuN-positive and NeuN-negative cells in all layers of the cerebral cortex (Fig. 1 A). The expression profiles of Smc3 and Smc1 transcripts in the developing brain were also examined (Fig. 1, B and C). The expression of both genes peaked at approximately embryonic day 15 (E15), when neuroblasts proliferate in the brain. Additionally, expression of both genes persisted in the brain until P8w, indicating that cohesin is expressed in post-mitotic neurons both during development and in adults.

To investigate the function of cohesin in neurons, we generated conditional Smc3-knockout mice. Mouse Smc3 contains 29 exons and spans ∼45.44 kb on chromosome 19. Smc3 was disrupted in embryonic stem (ES) cells by homologous recombination using a vector targeting exons 1 and 2 (Fig. 1 D). Targeted mutation of Smc3 was confirmed by Southern blot hybridization and PCR of genomic DNA (Fig. 1 E). We initially deleted the gene using the CAG-Cre mouse (RBRC01828; Matsumura et al., 2004), which expresses Cre recombinase in the embryo at the zygote stage. Among the resulting offspring, mice carrying the CAG-Cre transgene with a deleted copy of the Smc3 allele (CAG-Cre; Smc3+/−) were crossed with wild-type mice to remove the CAG-Cre transgene. The genotype of the offspring from an intercross of Smc3+/− mice did not exhibit a Mendelian ratio (Fig. 1 F). Furthermore, no Smc3−/− mice were detected at E10.5. Thus, Smc3 deficiency causes early embryonic lethality. We then characterized the Smc3+/− mice. Smc3 mRNA expression was reduced by 50% in the cortex of Smc3+/− mice compared with Smc3+/+ mice (Fig. 1 G). Smc3+/− mice exhibited no alterations in body weight or brain size and no apparent gross abnormal phenotype in the anatomical structure of the brain (Fig. 1, H and I).

We assessed whether homozygous knockout of Smc3 in specific cell types showed a similar phenotype as CdLS. We used Wnt1-Cre mice to delete Smc3 specifically in neural crest–derived tissue and assessed craniofacial abnormalities. Wnt1-Cre; Smc3 flox/flox mice showed cleft palate and hypoplasia of the skull (Fig. 2, A and B). Neuron-specific deletion of Smc3 using tau-Cre mice (tau-Cre; Smc3 flox/flox mice) resulted in growth retardation, and these mice died by P5w (Fig. 2 C). Ablation of Smc3 in nestin-expressing neural progenitors (Nestin-CreERT2; Smc3 flox/flox mice) caused enlarged ventricles (Fig. 2 D). Collectively, the phenotypes observed in these mice appear to be associated with the clinical signs of CdLS.

Cohesin regulates dendritic development in the cerebral cortex

We consider haplodeficient mice, which exhibit higher brain dysfunction, as a model of CdLS. Because abnormal synapse formation is observed in cortical layer II/III of some animal models of higher brain dysfunction (Hayashi et al., 2004; Cruz-Martín et al., 2010), we focused our analysis on individual pyramidal neurons in these layers. To investigate the effects of abnormally low Smc3 expression on neuronal morphology, we performed Golgi staining of brain tissue obtained from Smc3+/+ and Smc3+/− mice at P6w. Sholl analysis (Sholl, 1953) revealed that dendritic arbor complexity was greater in Smc3+/− mice than in Smc3+/+ mice (Fig. 3, A–C).

To identify whether the Smc3+/− phenotype resulted from abnormal neuronal Smc3, we generated neuron-specific heterozygous Smc3-knockout mice (TLCN-Cre; Smc3+/flox mice) and again observed greater dendritic arbor complexity (Fig. 3 D). This indicated that a sufficient level of neuronal Smc3 is required for proper dendritic development.

We then analyzed the structure of apical dendritic spines in cortical layer II/III at P8w. The percentage of mushroom-type mature spines was lower in the apical dendrites of Smc3+/− mice than in Smc3+/+ mice (Fig. 3 E). Additionally, the percent of thin- and filopodia-type immature spines was higher in the apical dendrites of Smc3+/− mice. Reflecting this structural alteration, spines were longer in Smc3+/− mice (Fig. 3 F). The percentage of stubby spines did not differ between Smc3+/+ and Smc3+/− mice. Additionally, the density of mushroom-type mature spines was significantly lower in Smc3+/− mice than in Smc3+/+ mice (Fig. 3, G and H). Thus, abnormally low expression of neuronal Smc3 led to anatomically perturbed synapse maturation in cortical layer II/III.

Reduced cohesin expression leads to impaired synapse maturation

The aforementioned morphological results prompted us to investigate the effect of cohesin on synapse formation. We observed a lower expression of the excitatory postsynaptic protein postsynaptic density 95 (PSD-95) in Smc3+/− mice than in Smc3+/+ mice but did not detect any significant change in the level of the excitatory presynaptic protein VGlut1 (Fig. 4, A and B). Furthermore, electron micrographic analysis revealed significantly thinner and shorter postsynaptic densities in Smc3+/− mice at P6w (Fig. 4, C–E). Thus, if the required level of cohesin is absent, proper development of synapses in the cerebral cortex cannot occur. These observations suggest that cohesin expression in terminally differentiated neurons must be strictly controlled for the formation of a precise neuronal network in the cerebral cortex during development.

Smc3+/− mice exhibit excessive anxiety-related behavior

Heterozygous mutations in cohesin or cohesin regulators are linked to CdLS, which is characterized by stunted growth, intellectual disability, and heightened anxiety. We therefore performed a battery of behavioral tests (Imayoshi et al., 2008) on Smc3+/− mice using adult male mice at least 11 wk old.

In the light/dark transition test (Takao and Miyakawa, 2006), whereas the distance traveled, time spent in the light box, and number of transitions did not differ between Smc3+/+ and Smc3+/− mice (Fig. 5, A–C), the latency of Smc3+/− mice to cross into the light box was significantly greater (Fig. 5 D), which suggests enhanced anxiety in Smc3+/− mice. We therefore further analyzed anxiety-like behavior. The marble-burying test is correlated with anxiety-like and repetitive behavior (Njung’e and Handley, 1991; Thomas et al., 2009). Compared with Smc3+/+ mice, Smc3+/− mice exhibited more marble-burying behavior, although the total distance traveled was similar between groups (Fig. 5, E and F). Novelty-induced hypophagia has also been used to measure anxiety-like behavior (Dulawa and Hen, 2005). In this test, mice are trained to approach a reward (sweetened condensed milk) in their home cage and are then placed in a novel, brightly lit cage. The latency to drinking and the volume of milk consumed are used as measures of anxiety-related behavior. Smc3+/− mice exhibited a significantly greater latency to drink in the novel cage compared with Smc3+/+ mice (Fig. 5, G–J). Thus, three separate tests indicate that, as in patients with CdLS (Basile et al., 2007; Richards et al., 2009; Nakanishi et al., 2012), low expression of cohesin leads to heightened anxiety-related behavior. We detected no significant differences in locomotion, nociception, social behavior, motor coordination, depression-like behavior, or spatial memory (Figs. S1, S2, and S3). Therefore, insufficient expression of cohesin induces a surprisingly specific behavioral phenotype, suggesting that it is required for the development of a neuronal network involved in specific higher brain function.

We further investigated whether decreased expression of neuronal Smc3 produced anxiety-like behavior. Neuron-specific heterozygous Smc3-knockout mice (tau-Cre; Smc3+/flox mice) also showed increased anxiety-like behavior in a novelty-induced hypophagia test (Fig. 6, A and B). However, the light/dark transition test or the marble-burying test did not detect the difference between control and tau-Cre; Smc3+/flox mice (Fig. 6, C–G). These results suggest that loss of cohesin function in neurons increases anxiety-like behavior, particularly in the novelty environment.

CdLS induces morphological changes in brain tissue

The anxiety-related findings suggest an analogy between the behavioral phenotype of mice with abnormally low cohesin expression and individuals with CdLS. However, the pathological features of the CdLS brain have not been thoroughly investigated. Thus, we next analyzed autopsied cortical brain tissue from CdLS that were stillborn at 39 wk of pregnancy by performing immunohistochemical analysis of dendritic arborization. Microtubule-associated protein 2 (MAP-2) staining revealed that the dendritic arbors in the CdLS brain were thin, tortuous, and fragmented (Fig. 7 A), consistent with our observations of dendritic arbors in Smc3+/− mice (Fig. 3 A). With Nissl staining, the density of neurons was lower in the brains of individuals with CdLS than in control stillborn individuals free of neurological disease (Fig. 7 B). We also performed immunohistochemistry for myelin basic protein (MBP), ionized calcium-binding adapter molecule 1 (Iba1), and glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP) to examine oligodendrocytes, microglia, and astrocytes, respectively. The density of processes on MBP-positive oligodendrocytes was lower in CdLS brains (Fig. 7 C), suggesting defective myelin formation. Iba1-positive cells were slightly rounder in CdLS brains, which suggested that microglia might have been activated (Fig. 7 D). Additionally, the number of GFAP-positive astrocytes was higher in CdLS brains. Intriguingly, most of the GFAP-positive cells in the CdLS brains had a greater number of processes and thus appeared to be activated (Fig. 7 E). These results suggest that attenuated cohesin function in the CdLS brain is associated with defective neuronal network formation with attenuated myelin formation and activation of microglia and astrocytes.

Cohesin regulates gene expression during CNS development

Because regulation of gene expression is considered to be an important function of cohesin in terminally differentiated cells (Wendt et al., 2008), we suspected that the phenotype associated with the neuron-specific Smc3-knockout mouse depends on cohesin-mediated gene expression during development. To test this hypothesis, we performed a gene expression analysis to identify the genes responsible for the phenotype, beginning with RNA-sequencing of cortical tissue samples from postnatal day 1 (P1) Smc3+/+ or Smc3+/− mice (Fig. 8 A and Table S1). We then focused on the time course of expression during dendritic formation (P1–P21). We found that in the cortex of Smc3+/− mice, many more genes were significantly altered relative to Smc3+/+ mice at P7 when synapse formation was dynamically occurred in the brain than at P1, P3, P14, or P21 (Fig. 8 B). Therefore, cohesin acts as a global gene regulator at a specific time point during development, which may explain why it plays such a specific role in cortical network formation.

To analyze the genes that exhibited altered expression during development, we performed clustering analysis in which 290 genes were hierarchically grouped and subsequently classified into four clusters (Fig. 8 C). To identify the characteristics of each cluster, we performed GO analysis. GO terms were significantly enriched in clusters 1, 2, and 4. The p-values of the GO enrichments in clusters 1 (P < 8.23e-05) and 2 (P < 6.76e-04) are shown in Fig. 8 D. The GO analysis for cluster 1 indicated enrichment for neuronal development, which might thus be responsible for the phenotype of Smc3+/− mice.

The GO analysis for cluster 2 indicated enrichment for immune processes, particularly for the response to interferons. Interferon signaling has been implicated in the regulation of dendritic and synaptic formation through STAT1 (Kim et al., 2002). Furthermore, we found that Stat1 expression was higher in the cortex of Smc3+/− mice at P21 (Table S2, cluster 2). Stat1 expression was also increased in the cortex of neuron-specific heterozygous Smc3-knockout (tau-Cre; Smc3+/flox) mice compared with Smc3 flox/flox mice (Fig. 9 A). However, the expression level of IFN-γ, which activates Stat1, was not different between Smc3 flox/flox and tau-Cre; Smc3+/flox mice (Fig. 9, B and C), suggesting that cohesin function seems to be required for the intracellular responses in neurons, but not for ligand expression.

These findings prompted us to examine whether knockdown of Stat1 could rescue the abnormal phenotype in Smc3+/− mice. To assess this, we looked at the number of cortical synapses in Smc3+/− mice. Transfection of Stat1 siRNA into cortical neurons led to lower STAT1 expression (Fig. 9 D). Cortical neurons were cultured for 21 d and then immunostained with anti–PSD-95 and anti-MAP2 antibodies. Numbers of PSD-95–positive puncta in cortical neurons were less in Smc3+/− mice than in wild-type mice, and this phenotype was partially rescued by Stat1 siRNA transfection (Fig. 9, E and F). These results suggest that cohesin regulates gene expression including STAT1 that eventually contributes to the maturation of synapses. As Stat1 knockdown only partially rescued phenotype, other molecules (e.g., the genes in clusters 1 and 2; Table S2) likely contribute to the Smc3+/− phenotype.

We further examined genome-wide histone modifications and the distribution of cohesin by chromatin immunoprecipitation (IP [ChIP]) with high-throughput sequencing (ChIP-seq). H3K4me3 is related to active promoters, and H3K27ac is related to both active promoters and enhancers (Shlyueva et al., 2014). We could not detect significant changes in the status of histone markers or the occupancy of the cohesin complex protein, Rad21 (Fig. 10 A). To enhance the positional resolution of ChIP-seq, we used spike-in samples. Rad21 distribution was altered inside but not outside differentially expressed genes (DEGs) of Smc3+/− mice at P14 (Fig. 10 B). Thus, lower cohesin occupancy inside the gene may alter gene expression during development, thus leading to impaired formation of dendrites and synapses.

We have demonstrated that the haplodeficiency of cohesin leads to defective dendrite development and synapse maturation in the cerebral cortex of mice. Moreover, mice with reduced cohesin expression exhibit heightened anxiety-related behavior. This phenotype is consistent with the part of symptoms of individuals with CdLS, who frequently exhibit intellectual disability, increased anxiety, and seizures. Furthermore, individuals with CdLS are frequently hyperactive (Berney et al., 1999), which was also observed in heterozygous Smc3-knockout mice during the dark part of the diurnal cycle in the home cage (Fig. S3, q and r). However, we could not detect the phenotype reflecting intellectual disability. To assess this point, we need to perform various behavior tests for learning and memory abilities. Because increased anxiety-like behavior was also detected in neuron-specific Smc3 heterozygous deficient mice (Fig. 6), the phenotype induced by decreased Smc3 may be caused by a cell-autonomous mechanism. Some anxiety-related behavior tests suggest increased anxiety in Smc3+/− mice, whereas other tests, such as the elevated plus maze, did not indicate increased anxiety. These findings suggest that the specific neuronal network seems to be affected by Smc3 deficiency. Altered dendritic and synapse formation was also observed in the prefrontal cortex, which regulates higher-order brain function, suggesting that the phenotype of Smc3+/− mice might be associated with disorganization in the prefrontal area.

In the current study, neuron-specific Smc3 knockout led to enhanced dendritic complexity. Analysis revealed that the abnormal neuronal and behavioral phenotype was related to altered control of gene expression that resulted from insufficient levels of cohesion. This result provides clear evidence of a critical role for cohesin in postmitotic neurons and confirms the existence of a chromatid-cohesin–independent function of cohesin in CNS development. Reduced cohesin in the brain leads to defective epigenetic control of several genes and results in specific neuronal and behavioral phenotypes. This finding suggests the importance of tight epigenetic regulation by cohesin at a specific time point in the developing brain. Thus, we have demonstrated a new epigenetic role for cohesion that affects development of neuronal networks.

Accumulating evidence suggests an association between dendritic abnormalities and higher-order brain function. Some genetic disorders, such as Down, Rett, and fragile X syndromes, are associated with dendrite and/or spine anomalies. In Down syndrome, dendrite branching and length are greater in infants compared with controls (Becker et al., 1986; Prinz et al., 1997), whereas branching, length, and spine density are all reduced in patients with Down syndrome (>30 yr old) compared with age-matched controls (Takashima et al., 1989; Becker et al., 1991). Dendritic arborization declines throughout life in individuals with Rett syndrome (Armstrong et al., 1995, 1998). Although neuronal density in the neocortex of individuals with fragile X syndrome is not different from normal controls, dendritic spines appear long and tortuous with prominent heads (Rudelli et al., 1985; Hinton et al., 1991). These variable synaptic complexity phenotypes might be consequences of altered formation, maturation, or pruning of synapses. In our experiment, greater dendritic arborization in the cortex of Smc3+/− mice might have resulted from deficient pruning of excess immature synapses or, alternatively, from compensating for fewer mature synapses. We cannot rule out the possibility that defective expression of factors that induce synapse formation could also have led to fewer mature synapses. To clarify these points, quantitative time course studies will be required.

We found that cohesin expression must be tightly controlled in the brain during development, and lower cohesin levels in the brain lead to global changes in gene expression at P7, which results in a specific neuronal and behavioral phenotype. Although the distribution change of cohesin in Smc3+/− mice was modest (Fig. 10), the occupancy of cohesin inside DEGs was decreased, and this may lead to the disruption of precise gene expression during the development of CNS. Interestingly, the expression of immune response–related genes, especially those related to the “response to interferons,” was altered in the brains of Smc3+/− mice (Fig. 8 D). Interferon-γ participates in the regulation of dendrite and synapse formation via STAT1 (Kim et al., 2002). We therefore postulate that cohesin supports dendrite and synapse formation at least partially through the regulation of the response of interferon-γ.

Furthermore, the disruption of epigenetic regulation leads to the up-regulation of immune genes and abnormal synaptic function (Feng et al., 2010). Deficiencies in the DNA methyltransferases DNMT1 and DNMT3 result in greater expression of immune genes, including STAT1, and lead to abnormal long-term plasticity (Feng et al., 2010). The up-regulation of STAT1 may be associated with synaptic plasticity (Tropea et al., 2006). We observed increased STAT1 expression in the cortex of Smc3+/− mice at P21 (Table S2, cluster 2). Additionally, several unregulated genes in Smc3+/− mice are shared by those in Dnmt1/Dnmt3 double-knockout mice (Feng et al., 2010). Perturbations in DNA methylation may be relevant to intellectual disability in humans. Cells from patients with CdLS exhibit altered DNA methylation and cohesin binding (Liu et al., 2010). These observations suggest that cohesin regulates neuronal development in part through the regulation of immune gene expression. The disturbed synapse formation in Smc3+/− mice was partially, but not completely, rescued by STAT1 knockdown. These findings suggest that multiple genes contribute to these brain phenotypes and that cohesin strictly regulates the expression level of these genes.

In summary, decreased cohesin levels in mice led to defective dendrite and synapse formation in the brain and increased anxiety-like behavior. Many genes are transcriptionally controlled by cohesin, revealing a regulatory system of chromosomal architecture that is broadly responsible for neuronal network formation. Further studies will reveal specific temporal and spatial changes of chromosomal architecture during brain development.

Mice

Mice were bred and maintained in the Institute of Experimental Animal Sciences at Osaka University Graduate School of Medicine. All experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Committee of Osaka University.

A targeting vector in a pSP72 (Promega) vector backbone was constructed by first subcloning a 9.91-kb Smc3-specific genomic region from the 129/SvEv BAC clone RP23: 146L13. The region was designed such that the short homology arm extends 1.72 kb 3′ of exon 2, and the 5.75-kb-long homology arm ends 5′ of exon 1. A PGK-Frt/loxP-Neo cassette was inserted 177 bp downstream of exon 2, and a single loxP site was inserted 1.14 kb upstream of exon 1. The target region was 2.43 kb and included exons 1 and 2. The targeting vector was linearized with AscI and introduced into BA1 (C57BL/6 × 129/SvEv) hybrid ES cells by electroporation (inGenious Targeting Laboratory).

After selection with G418 antibiotic, the surviving clones were amplified by PCR and used for Southern blot analysis to identify recombinant ES clones. For Southern blot analysis, genomic DNA from the ES clones was digested with AvrII and BsrGI and hybridized with a probe targeted at either of the 5′ external regions, as indicated in Fig. 1. Five correctly targeted ES cell clones were identified among 200 neomycin-resistant colonies. Two mutated ES clones (#121 and #142) were microinjected into C57BL/6 blastocysts, and a colony of Smc3+/flox mice was successfully established from clone #121. Chimeric males (F0) were obtained and mated with C57BL/6J females to transmit the modified Smc3 allele to the germline. To remove the Neo cassette, F1 heterozygotes were crossed with FLPe mice (B6.Cg-Tg(ACTFLPe)9205Dym/J, stock #003800; The Jackson Laboratory), generating offspring with a conditional Smc3 loxP allele (F2). The mice were backcrossed at least seven generations to the C57BL/6J strain (Charles River) for use in experiments.

Genotypes were determined by PCR of mouse tail DNA using NDEL1-F (5′-GGAAAGAATGTTGCCCTAGAAGT-3′) and NDEL2-R (5′-CGCTAGGCAAACATTCCACTAC-3′) for the wild-type (554 bp) and mutant (374 bp) alleles and LOX1-F (5′-GGTGGAATTTGGCCAGAGTTTCAG-3′), SDL2-R (5′-AATAGGTAGGTACTAGGGTCAGTCCC-3′), and AT2-R (5′-CTTAACTTACACCACTGGGCTCCA-3′) for the wild-type (434 bp) and mutant (373 bp) alleles.

To generate Smc3+/− mice, homozygous floxed mice (Smc3 flox/flox) were mated with B6.Cg-Tg(CAG-Cre)CZ-MO2Osb. (pCAGGS-Cre recombinase) transgenic mice (RBRC01828; Matsumura et al., 2004). Offspring carrying CAG-Cre and “floxed” (flanked by loxP) Smc3 were backcrossed for at least seven generations with the wild-type C57BL/6 background. The viability of homozygous Smc3-knockout mice was examined by intercrossing Smc3+/− mice (Fig. 1 F). Smc3+/+ mice were used as a control.

To generate neuron-specific Smc3-knockout mice, Smc3 flox/flox mice were crossed with telencephalin (TLCN)-Cre line D transgenic mice (Mitsui et al., 2007) to obtain offspring heterozygous for the floxed Smc3 allele and hemizygous for the TLCN-Cre allele (TLCN-Cre; Smc3+/flox). To obtain homozygous floxed Smc3, TLCN-Cre; Smc3+/flox mice were crossed with Smc3 flox/flox mice. Smc3 flox/flox mice were used as a control.

Wnt1-Cre mice were obtained from The Jackson Laboratory (stock number 009107). tau-Cre mice were provided by A. Harada (Osaka University, Osaka, Japan; Muramatsu et al., 2008). Nestin-CreERT2 mice were provided by R. Kageyama (Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan; Imayoshi et al., 2006). 4 mg tamoxifen was administered by oral gavage into pregnant mice at E10.5. The pups were fixed with 4% PFA at E18.5.

The Animal Research Committee of the Osaka University Graduate School of Medicine approved this study. All experiments were performed in accordance with the guidelines for animal experimentation from the Animal Research Committee of the Osaka University Graduate School of Medicine (permit number 24-067-005). All in vivo studies were performed under anesthesia.

Behavioral analysis

Behavioral analyses were performed as previously described.

Animals and experimental design

Adult male mice at least 11 wk old were used for all behavioral analyses. Mice were housed under a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 a.m.) with ad libitum access to food and water. Behavioral testing was performed between 9:00 a.m. and 5:00 p.m. After each test, the apparatus was cleaned with super-hypochlorous water to prevent bias caused by olfactory cues. The same mouse batch (group of mice) was used in all tests; all behavioral tests were separated from each other by at least 1 d. The raw data from the behavioral tests are not presented here but are available in the gene-brain-phenotyping database (http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/). All behavioral testing procedures have been approved by the National Institute for Physiological Sciences (Okazaki, Japan).

General health and neurological screen (Miyakawa et al., 2001)

The righting, whisker touch, and ear twitch reflexes were evaluated. Several physical features, including the presence of whiskers or bald patches in the coat, were also recorded.

Light/dark transition test (Takao and Miyakawa, 2006)

The apparatus used for the light/dark transition test was a cage (21 × 42 × 25 cm) divided into two sections of equal size by a partition with a door (O’Hara & Co.). One chamber was brightly illuminated (390 lux), whereas the other chamber was dark (2 lux). Mice were placed in the dark chamber and allowed to move freely between the two chambers with the door open for 10 min. The latency to first entry into the lit chamber, number of transitions, total distance traveled, and time spent in each chamber were recorded using ImageLD software (O’Hara & Co.; see Data analysis).

Open field test

Locomotor activity was measured using an open field test. Each mouse was placed in the corner of the open field apparatus (40 × 40 × 30 cm; Accuscan Instruments). The test chamber was illuminated at 100 lux. The total distance traveled, vertical activity (rearing measured by counting the number of photobeam interruptions), time spent in the center area (20 × 20 cm), and beam-break counts for stereotyped behavior were recorded. If an animal breaks the same beam (or set of beams) repeatedly, the monitor judges this behavior as stereotypy (as typically occurs during grooming and head bobbing, for example). The stereotypy count was the number of beam breaks that occurred during a period of stereotypic activity. Resting time was calculated as the difference between the total time and the time spent moving. The data were collected for 120 min.

Elevated plus maze test (Komada et al., 2008)

The elevated plus maze test consisted of two open arms (25 × 5 cm) and two enclosed arms of the same size with 15-cm-high transparent walls (O’Hara & Co.). The arms and central square were made of white plastic plates and were elevated 55 cm above the floor. To minimize the likelihood of animals falling from the apparatus, 3-mm-high Plexiglas walls surrounded the sides of the open arms. Arms of the same type were located opposite from each other. Each mouse was placed in the central square of the maze (5 × 5 cm), facing one of the closed arms. Mouse behavior was recorded during a 10-min test period. The number of entries into an arm and the time spent in the open and enclosed arms were recorded. The percentage of entries into open arms, time spent in open arms, number of total entries, and total distance traveled were analyzed. The data acquisition and analysis were performed automatically using ImageEP software (see Data analysis).

Rotarod test (Tsujimura et al., 2008)

Motor coordination and balance were tested using an accelerating rotarod (Ugo Basile North America). The test was performed by placing mice on rotating drums (3 cm in diameter) and measuring the time each animal was able to maintain its balance on the rod. The speed of the rotarod was accelerated from 4 rpm to 40 rpm over a 5-min period. The animals performed three trials per day on two consecutive days, and trials were separated by intervals of more than 1 h.

Hot plate test (Tsujimura et al., 2008)

The hot plate test was used to evaluate sensitivity to a painful stimulus. Mice were placed on a 55.0 ± 0.3°C hot plate (Columbus Instruments International), and the latency to the first hind paw response was recorded. The hind paw response was defined as either a foot shake or a paw lick.

Social interaction test in a novel environment

In the social interaction test, two mice of identical genotype that had previously been housed in different cages were placed in a box together (40 × 40 × 30 cm) and allowed to explore freely for 10 min (Tanda et al., 2009). Because a pair of mice was used as a sample in the test, the number of samples was half the usual number. Social behavior was monitored with a charge-coupled device camera connected to a Macintosh computer. Analysis was performed automatically using ImageSI software (see Data analysis). The total number of contacts, total duration of active contacts, total contact duration, mean duration per contact, and total distance traveled were recorded. If the two mice contacted each other and the distance traveled by either mouse was longer than 10 cm, the behavior was considered an “active contact.” Images were captured at three frames per second, and the distance traveled between two successive frames was calculated for each mouse.

Crawley’s sociability and preference for social novelty test

The test for sociability and preference for social novelty is a well-designed method (Crawley, 2004; Moy et al., 2004). The apparatus comprised a rectangular, three-chambered box with a lid containing an infrared video camera (O’Hara & Co.). Each chamber was 20 × 40 × 22 cm. The clear Plexiglas dividing walls had small openings (5 × 3 cm) to allow access from the middle chamber into each side chamber. In a corner of each side chamber was a small, circular wire cage (11-cm height, 9-cm diameter, 0.5-cm space between bars) that allowed nose contact between the bars but prevented fighting. A habituation session was not performed in the apparatus. An unfamiliar C57BL/6J male mouse (stranger 1) that had no prior contact with the subject mouse was placed in the cage in one of the side chambers of the cage. The placement of stranger 1 in the left or right side of the chamber was systematically alternated between trials. The subject mouse was then placed in the middle chamber and allowed to explore the entire social test box for 10 min. The amount of time spent in each chamber and within 5 cm of each wire cage was measured with the aid of a camera fitted on top of the box. After the first test, each mouse was tested in a second 10-min session to quantify social preference for a new stranger. A second, unfamiliar mouse (stranger 2) was placed in the cage of the chamber that had been empty during the first 10-min session. The test mouse thus had a choice between the first, already-investigated unfamiliar mouse (stranger 1) and the novel unfamiliar mouse (stranger 2). As in the first test, the amount of time spent in each chamber and within 5 cm of each wire cage during the 10-min session was recorded. All stranger mice were C57BL/6J male mice and were not littermates. Analysis was performed automatically using ImageCSI software (see Data analysis).

Prepulse inhibition/acoustic startle response (Tanda et al., 2009)

A startle reflex measurement system was used (O'Hara & Co.). A test session began by placing a mouse in a Plexiglas cylinder, where it was left undisturbed for 10 min. White noise was used as the startle stimulus, and the duration was 40 ms for all trial types. The startle response was recorded for 140 ms (measuring the response every 1 ms) starting with the onset of the prepulse stimulus. The level of background noise in each chamber was 70 dB. The peak startle amplitude recorded during the 140-ms sampling window was used as the dependent variable. A test session consisted of six trial types (i.e., two for startle stimulus-only trials, and four for prepulse inhibition trials). The intensity of the startle stimulus was 110 or 120 dB. The prepulse sound was presented 100 ms before the startle stimulus, and its intensity was 74 or 78 dB. Four combinations of prepulse and startle stimuli were used (74–110 dB, 78–110 dB, 74–120 dB, and 78–120 dB). Six blocks of the six trial types were presented in a pseudorandom order such that each trial type was presented once within a block. The mean intertrial interval was 15 s (range, 10–20 s).

Porsolt forced swim test (Takao et al., 2008)

To assess the depression-like behavior, we performed Porsolt forced test. The apparatus consisted of four Plexiglas cylinders (20 cm high × 10 cm diameter). An opaque panel separated the cylinders to prevent the mice from seeing each other (O’Hara & Co.). The cylinders were filled with water (23°C) to a height of 7.5 cm. A mouse was placed into the cylinder, and immobility and distance traveled were recorded over a 10-min test period. Images were captured at one frame per second. For each pair of successive frames, the area (pixels) within which the mouse moved was measured. When the area was below a certain threshold, the mouse was considered “immobile.” When the area equaled or exceeded this threshold, the mouse was considered “moving.” The optimal threshold was determined by adjusting it to the amount of immobility measured by human observation. Immobility lasting less than 2 s was not included in the analysis. Retention tests were administered 24 h after training. Data acquisition and analysis were performed automatically using ImagePS software (see Data analysis).

Barnes circular maze test (Imayoshi et al., 2008)

Barnes maze is a widely used behavioral test measuring spatial reference memory. The test was conducted on “dry land”—a white circular surface, 1.0 m in diameter, with 12 holes equally spaced around the perimeter (O’Hara & Co.). The circular open field was elevated 75 cm from the floor. A black Plexiglas escape box (17 × 13 × 7 cm) with paper cage bedding on the bottom was located under one of the holes. The hole above the escape box represented the target. The location of the target was consistent for a given mouse but was randomized across mice. The maze was rotated daily, with the spatial location of the target unchanged with respect to the distal visual room cues, to prevent a bias based on olfactory or proximal cues within the maze. Three trials per day were conducted for six successive days. On day 7, a probe trial was conducted without the escape box to confirm that this spatial task was acquired based on navigation using distal environment room cues. One trial was conducted immediately after the probe test, and another probe trial was conducted 1 wk later. The number of errors to reach the target hole and the time spent around each hole were recorded using ImageBM software (see Data analysis).

T-maze spontaneous alternation task (Ohira et al., 2013)

The spontaneous alternation task was conducted using a modified T-maze apparatus and an automated video-tracking system (O’Hara & Co.; Shoji et al., 2012). The apparatus was constructed of white plastic runways with 25-cm-high walls and was partitioned into six areas by sliding doors that could be automatically opened by sliding downward. The stem of the T was designated as area S2 (13 × 24 cm), and the arms of the T were designated as areas A1 and A2 (11.5 × 20.5 cm). Areas P1 and P2 were the connecting passageways from the arms (A1 or A2) to the start compartment (S1). Mice were placed in S1 and immediately subjected to a forced-choice run (pseudorandomly assigned to either the left or the right arm). Mice were held in one of the arms (A1 or A2) for 10 s. The door was then opened so that the mice could return to the start area (S1). They were held in S1 for 3 s and then subjected to a free-choice run in which they were given access to both arms. This sequence (trial) was repeated 10 times per day (cut-off time 7,200 s). The intertrial interval was 60 s. The percentage of trials in which mice entered the arm opposite the forced-choice run during the free-choice run was calculated as the percentage of correct responses. Data acquisition and data analysis were performed using ImageTM software (see Data Analysis).

Tail suspension test (Tsujimura et al., 2008)

The tail suspension test was performed over a 10-min test session according to cited procedures (Tsujimura et al., 2008). Mice were suspended 30 cm above the floor in a visually isolated area by adhesive tape that was placed ∼1 cm from the tip of the tail, and their behavior was recorded over a 10-min test period. Data acquisition and analysis were performed automatically using ImageTS software (see Data analysis).

Contextual and cued fear conditioning test (Tsujimura et al., 2008; Shoji et al., 2014)

On the training day (day 1), each mouse was placed in a test chamber (26 × 34 × 29 cm) inside a sound-attenuated room and allowed to explore freely for 2 min. A 60-dB white noise served as the conditioned stimulus and was presented for 30 s, followed by a mild (2 s, 0.3 mA) foot shock, which served as the unconditioned stimulus. Two more conditioned stimulus–unconditioned stimulus pairings were presented with a 2-min interstimulus interval. On day 2, context testing was conducted in the same chamber. Cued testing with altered context was conducted after conditioning using a triangular box (35 × 35 × 40 cm) made of white opaque Plexiglas, which was located in a different room. Data acquisition, control of stimuli (i.e., tones and shocks), and data analysis were performed automatically using ImageFZ software (see Data analysis). Images were captured at one frame per second. For each pair of successive frames, the area (pixels) in which the mouse moved was calculated. When this area was below a certain threshold (20 pixels), the behavior was considered “freezing.” When the area equaled or exceeded the threshold, the behavior was considered “nonfreezing.” The optimal threshold (number of pixels) was determined by adjusting to the amount of freezing measured by human observation. Freezing that lasted less than the defined threshold (2 s) was not included in the analysis.

Gait analysis (Yao et al., 2011)

The gait of adult mice during spontaneous walk/trot locomotion at velocities of 24 cm/s was analyzed using simultaneous video and reaction force analysis. Equivalent stride times for forepaws and hind paws were composed of a shorter stance and a longer swing time. Peak vertical reaction force increases with decreasing stance time, and that for the forepaws was ∼5% greater than that for the hind paws across the whole stance time range studied.

Marble-burying test

The marble-burying test was modified from a previous study (Njung’e and Handley, 1991). Mice were individually placed in transparent polycarbonate cages (12 × 27 × 9 cm) containing a 5-cm layer of fine bedding material (SLC; Hamamatsu Photonics) and 25 equally spaced glass marbles (1.5 cm in diameter). The number of marbles buried and the total distance traveled were recorded with a video camera for 30 min. When half the marble was in the fine bedding material, it was operationally defined as buried.

Novelty-induced hypophagia test

Anxiety-related behavior was also tested using the novelty-induced hypophagia test originally developed by Hen et al. (Dulawa and Hen, 2005; Meshi et al., 2006). Training consisted of daily sessions held for three consecutive days in which group-housed mice were exposed for 30 min daily to diluted (25%) sweetened condensed milk (Morinaga) from sippers attached to 5-ml pipettes in each cage (with food and drinking water available ad libitum). On day 4, mice were divided into two groups. In home-cage testing, mice were removed from their home cage and placed into a holding cage. After 30 min, one mouse was returned to the home cage fitted with a filled pipette under dim lighting (50 lux). The latency to drink and the volume consumed were recorded every 5 min for 30 min. For testing in the novel environment, mice were placed into new clean cages fitted with a filled pipette, white paper flooring (without shavings), and bright lighting (600 lux) provided by a 60-W light source located above the floor. The latency to drink and the volume consumed were recorded every 5 min for 30 min.

24-h monitoring in the home cage

A system that automatically analyzes the locomotor activity of mice in the home cage was used (INNOVIVE). The system contains a home cage (29 × 18 × 12 cm) and a filter cage top, with an infrared video camera attached to the top of a 13-cm-high metal stand. Two mice of the same genotype that had been housed separately were placed together in a home cage. Their locomotor activity and social behavior were monitored for 1 wk. Output from the video camera was fed into a computer, and images were captured at a rate of one frame per second. Distance traveled was measured automatically using ImageHA software. Social interaction was measured by counting the number of particles detected in each frame: two particles indicated that the mice were not in contact with each other, and one particle (i.e., the tracking software could not distinguish two separate bodies) indicated contact between the mice. Using the same system, circadian rhythms of locomotor activity were also analyzed. Each mouse was individually housed for 1 wk under a 12-h light/dark cycle followed by 1 wk in constant darkness. Distance traveled was automatically measured and analyzed using ImageHA software (see Data analysis).

Data analysis

Behavioral data were obtained automatically by applications based on the public-domain ImageJ 1.46 program and modified for each test by Tsuyoshi Miyakawa (available through O’Hara & Co.). ImageLD, ImageEP, ImageFZ, ImageTM, and ImageHA are freely available at http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/software.html. Statistical analysis was performed using StatView (SAS Institute). The data were analyzed using a paired Student’s t test, one-way ANOVA, or two-way repeated-measures ANOVA. Values in graphs are expressed as the mean ± SEM.

Golgi staining and Sholl analysis

Mice were maintained at a specific pathogen–free animal room in the Institute of Experimental Animal Sciences, Osaka University Graduate School of Medicine. Mice were housed three to five per cage in a room with a 12-h light/dark cycle with access to food and water ad libitum. Brains from P3w or P6w Smc3+/− mice, TLCN-Cre; Smc3+/flox mice, and gender-matched littermate-control mice were prepared using the standard Golgi-Cox impregnation technique using the FD Rapid GolgiStain kit (FD NeuroTechnologies, Inc.). Mice were anesthetized, and brain tissues were collected. 200-µm serial coronal sections were collected from control and Smc3 mutant mice. In each hemisphere, pyramidal neurons with the soma in layer II/III were selected in the prefrontal cortex, which has been shown the relationship to higher-order brain function (bregma +2.10 to +0.70 mm; Paxinos and Franklin, 1997) and subsequently analyzed under higher magnification. Twelve cells were traced per mouse. Only fully impregnated pyramidal neurons that displayed dendritic trees without obvious truncation and that were isolated from neighboring impregnated neurons, were retained for analysis. For each selected neuron, the neuronal arbor was traced under an oil-immersion lens on a motorized microscope connected to a computer running Neurolucida Software (MBF Bioscience). A 40× lens was used to measure dendrites, and a 100× oil lens was used to observe spine morphology. The slides were examined by two masked observers. Sholl analysis was performed using NeuroExplorer (MBF Bioscience), and branch length, number of branches, neuronal complexity, spine length, and spine density were measured and analyzed. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA was used for Sholl analysis. Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05. We used a minimal sample for this experiment in accordance with the National Centre for the Replacement, Refinement and Reduction of Animals in Research (London, UK).

Histological analysis

Cryostat sections were incubated with blocking solution containing 5% bovine serum albumin and 0.1%–0.3% Triton X-100 in PBS for 1 h at room temperature and then incubated overnight with the appropriate antibodies at 4°C. Immunoreactivity was visualized using fluorescence-conjugated secondary antibodies. Sections were then mounted with coverslips and mounting medium. Antibodies to Smc3 (1:500; Abcam) and NeuN (1:2,000; EMD Millipore) were used. For Nissl staining, 20-µm-thick coronal brain sections prepared from Smc3+/+ or Smc3+/− mice were stained with cresyl violet (MP Biomedicals). Skeletal samples prepared from P0.5 mice were stained with alizarin red and alcian blue using standard techniques.

Microscope image acquisition

Images were acquired on a confocal microscope (FV1100) using FV10-ASW software (Olympus).

Immunohistochemistry of autopsied brain tissue

The ethical committee of the Osaka Medical Center and Research Institute for Maternal and Child Health reviewed and approved the protocol for analysis of autopsied brain tissue. Brain tissue from stillborns with CdLS was obtained after death at 39 wk of pregnancy. Control brain tissue was obtained from stillborns free of neurological diseases after death at 40 wk of pregnancy. Tissues were fixed with 10% formalin and then embedded in paraffin. 6-µm-thick brain sections were cut, deparaffinized with xylene, and then rehydrated in ethanol. Sections were boiled in citric acid buffer and then subjected to immunofluorescence staining with antibodies to MAP-2 (1:500; Sigma-Aldrich), MBP (1:200; Abcam), Iba1 (1:200; Wako Pure Chemical Industries), and GFAP (1:200; Sigma-Aldrich). Sections were then washed with PBS and incubated with an Alexa Fluor 568–conjugated secondary antibody (1:1,000; Molecular Probes) at room temperature for 1 h.

Electron microscopy

Mice were deeply anesthetized and transcardially perfused with PBS, pH 7.4, followed by ice-cold 2.5% glutaraldehyde in phosphate buffer, pH 7.4. The brains were removed, and the cortices were dissected and postfixed in glutaraldehyde solution at 4°C for 3 d. The samples were washed more than three times for 10 min each in 0.1 M phosphate buffer and then postfixed in 1% osmium tetroxide for 1 h at 4°C. Next, the samples were washed three times in distilled water for 10 min each. After en-bloc staining in 0.5% uranyl acetate in distilled water for 4 h to overnight, the samples were washed three times in distilled water for 10 min each. Samples were dehydrated using serial dilutions of ethanol (50%, 80%, 90%, 95%, and 100%) for 10 min each. Samples were then treated twice with propylene oxide for 20 min each and impregnated with 50:50 propylene oxide:epoxy resin overnight at room temperature. Finally, samples were impregnated with 100% epoxy resin, embedded in molds, and incubated for 48 h at 60°C. 1-µm semithin sections were then cut on an ultramicrotome (Reichert Ultracut E) and stained with 0.5% toluidine blue. From these, 80-nm thin striatal sections were cut on an ultramicrotome, mounted on 200-mesh copper grids, and poststained in 2% uranyl acetate in distilled water for 15 min and in Sato’s lead citrate stain for 7 min. Grids were examined by transmission electron microscopy (Hitachi H-7650). Images were acquired at 4,000× magnification using a Hamamatsu Photonics C4742-57-12ER CCD-based digital camera and Hitachi H-7650 control software, version 01.02. Postsynaptic densities were measured using ImageJ software (National Institutes of Health) by an observer who was blinded to the genotype of the samples.

In situ hybridization

The cDNA fragments of mouse IFN-γ were amplified by RT-PCR using previously described in situ hybridization primers (Kimura et al., 2006). The amplified fragments were cloned into the pCR-blunt II-TOPO vector (Invitrogen). Serial sagittal sections, 16 µM thick were mounted on APS-coated glass slides (Matsunami Glass) and processed for in situ hybridization as previously described (Fujiwara et al., 2016).

Western blotting

Mouse cortices were homogenized in lysis buffer containing 50 mM Tris-HCl, pH 7.5, 150 mM NaCl, 1 mM EDTA, 1.0% NP-40, and a protease inhibitor cocktail (Roche). Cell lysates were boiled in sample buffer for 5 min. The proteins were separated by SDS-PAGE and transferred onto polyvinylidene difluoride membranes (EMD Millipore). The membrane was blocked with 5% nonfat dry milk in PBS containing 0.05% Tween-20 (PBS-T) and then incubated with primary antibody diluted in PBS-T containing 1% nonfat dry milk for 1 h at room temperature or overnight at 4°C. After washing in PBS-T, the membrane was incubated with horseradish peroxidase–conjugated secondary antibodies. An enhanced chemiluminescence system (GE Healthcare) was used for detection. Signals were detected and quantified using an LAS-3000 image analyzer (Fuji Film). Class III β-tubulin was used as a loading control.

RNA extraction, reverse transcription, and real-time PCR

Total RNA was extracted from astrocytes or the cortex with TRIzol (Invitrogen) and reverse transcribed using the High-Capacity cDNA Reverse Transcription kit (Applied Biosystems). mRNA expression was determined using a 7300 fast real-time PCR system (Applied Biosystems). TaqMan assays (Applied Biosystems) were used to quantitate Smc1 (Mm 00484015_m1) and Smc3 (Mm 00490624_m1) using the TaqMan Gene Expression Master Mix (Applied Biosystems). The SYBR Green assay was performed to quantify Stat1 and IFN-γ expression. Primers for mouse Stat1 and IFN-γ were designed using Primer Express software (Applied Biosystems). The relative mRNA expression was calculated after normalization to the expression of glyceraldehyde 3-phosphate dehydrogenase (GAPDH) mRNA. Cycle threshold values (Ct values) were calculated by the ΔΔCt method to obtain fold differences.

RNA extraction, cDNA library preparation, and RNA-sequencing

Total RNA was extracted from mouse cortex. The cDNA library was prepared using TruSeq RNA Sample Prep kit v2 (Illumina) for P1 samples, and TruSeq Stranded Total RNA with Ribo-Zero Gold kit (Illumina) for P3–P21 samples. All RNA-sequencing experiments were run on an Illumina HiSeq-2000, and RNA samples were sequenced with four replicates for P1 samples and three replicates for P3–P21 samples, according to the standard Illumina protocol, to create 31–65 × 106 single-end reads.

Read alignment and transcriptome processing

RNA-sequencing data analysis was performed with TopHat2 version 2.0.9 (Kim et al., 2013) and Cufflinks version 2.2.1 (Trapnell et al., 2013). RNA-sequencing reads for each library were independently mapped to the mouse reference sequence (NCBIM37/mm9) using TopHat2 with the options “-N 2 -G, Ensembl.gtf” (http://www.ensembl.org/info/data/ftp/index.html), which contains the annotations of both coding and noncoding genes. Further analysis to reveal differential expression in annotations and novel transcripts was performed with Cufflinks.

GO analysis

Enrichment analysis of the GO terms for DEGs was performed using g:Profiler (Reimand et al., 2011). Among 382 DEGs, 290 genes with enough alignment for testing (test status was “OK”) were chosen for the clustering analysis after removing three sex-specific genes.

ChIP-seq analysis

ChIP was performed as previously described (Izumi et al., 2015) except for homogenizing the fixed cells in 1% formaldehyde. LB1 (20 mM Tris-HCl, pH 7.5, 10 mM NaCl, 1 mM EDTA, 0.2% NP-40, 5 mM DTT, 1 mM PMSF). The cells were subjected to 10 strokes in a dounce homogenizer (Komata et al., 2014). Antibodies used for ChIP were as follows. Anti–histone H3 lysine-27 acetylation (H3K27Ac; Stasevich et al., 2014) and H3 lysine-4 trimethylation (H3K4me3) antibodies were provided by H. Kimura (Tokyo Institute of Technology, Tokyo, Japan). We also used antibody against Rad21, which has been described previously (Minamino et al., 2015). DNA from input and ChIP fractions were processed and sequenced using the Illumina HiSeq-2000 system, according to manufacturer’s instructions. In brief, DNA was sheared to an mean size of ∼150 bp by ultrasonication (Covaris), end-repaired, ligated to sequencing adapters, amplified, size-selected, and sequenced to generate single-end 66-bp reads. Sequenced reads were aligned to the mouse reference sequence (mm9) using Bowtie2 version 2.1.0 (Langmead and Salzberg, 2012) with default options. After filtering duplicated reads, peak calling and ChIP-seq data visualization were performed by DROMPA version 2.5.1 (Nakato et al., 2013). To calibrate ChIP-seq data for Rad21, we used the method described by Hu et al. (2015). The whole-cell extract from the mouse cortex was mixed with whole-cell extract from human RPE cells, and then ChIP was performed as described. Sequenced reads were aligned to mouse (mm9) and human (hg19) reference sequences. ChIP-seq profiles were multiplied by the occupancy ratio (OR) as previously described (Hu et al., 2015). The OR is calculated as follows: OR = (WhIPm)/(WmIPh), where Wm and IPm are the number of mapped reads to only the mouse genome from input and IP fractions; Wh and IPh are the number of mapped reads to only the human genome from input and IP fractions.

Cell culture and siRNA

Mouse Stat1 siRNA (stealth siRNA; Invitrogen) was used for knockdown experiments. Negative control siRNA (Ambion) with no sequence similarity to mouse genes was used as a control siRNA.

P1 mouse cortices were isolated and dissociated with 0.25% trypsin (Invitrogen) and 0.5 mg/ml DNase1 (Sigma-Aldrich) for 15 min at 37°C. DMEM (Invitrogen) containing 10% FBS was added, and the cells were centrifuged at 1,000 rpm for 4 min. Neurons were plated on poly-l-lysine–coated chamber slides and maintained in DMEM/F12 medium containing B27 supplement (Invitrogen) at 37°C in an atmosphere of 5% CO2. We obtained C57BL/6J mice (P1) from Japan SLC, Inc. The mice were bred and maintained in the Institute of Experimental Animal Sciences at Osaka University Graduate School of Medicine. The neurons were transfected with siRNA using a 4D-Nucleofector (Lonza). The cells were washed and resuspended in room temperature Nucleofector Solution. The cell–Nucleofector solution complex and siRNAs were then gently mixed and transferred into a cuvette, followed by nucleofection. Knockdown efficacy was analyzed after 72 h by real-time PCR. For immunocytochemistry, the cells were cultured for 21 d and fixed with 4% paraformaldehyde for 30 min. The cells were then permeabilized, and nonspecific sites were blocked by incubating with PBS containing 0.1% Triton X-100 and 5% BSA. The cells were incubated with anti-MAP2 (1:2,000; Sigma-Aldrich) and anti-PSD-95 (1:500; Thermo Fisher Scientific) antibodies diluted in a blocking solution overnight at 4°C. The cells were then washed in PBS and incubated with fluorescence-conjugated secondary antibodies for 1 h at room temperature. Because there was variability in PSD-95 punctate signals on neurons from the culture preparations, the number of PSD-95 signals from each group was normalized to the control group (Smc3+/+ neurons transfected with control siRNA).

Online supplemental material

Figs. S1 and S2 show the results of general behavioral tests and social behavior tests, respectively. Fig. S3 demonstrates the results of memory tests and locomotor activity in the home cage. Tables S1 and S2 are included as Excel files and show RNA-sequencing results and gene clustering results, respectively.

We thank Masaru Okabe (Research Institute for Microbial Diseases, Osaka University) for the gift of CAG-Cre mice, Yoshihiro Yoshihara and Sachiko Mitsui (Laboratory for the Neurobiology of the Synapse, RIKEN Brain Science Institute) for the gift of TLCN-Cre mice and helpful suggestions, Ryoichiro Kageyama and Itaru Imayoshi (Institute for Virus Research, Kyoto University) for the gift of Nestin-Cre ERT2 mice, and Akihiro Harada (Osaka University) for the gift of tau-Cre mice. We are also grateful to Eiji Oiki (Center for Medical Research and Education, Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka University) for technical support with electron microscopy.

This work was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS; to Y. Fujita), a JSPS Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (JSPS 25221309; to T. Yamashita), a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas (Chromosome OS; to K. Shirahige), the Platform Project for Supporting in Drug Discovery and Life Science Research from the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED; to K. Shirahige), and a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas (Comprehensive Brain Science Network) from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan to K. Takao.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Author contributions: Y. Fujita, K. Shirahige, and T. Yamashita conceived the project and designed the experiments. Y. Fujita and T. Yamashita wrote the paper. Y. Fujita generated the Smc3+/flox mice, performed the experiments, and analyzed the data. M. Bando, R. Nakato, K. Masuda, and Y. Katou contributed to the RNA-sequencing analysis. Y. Fujita, Takashi Tanaka, K. Takao, and T. Miyakawa contributed to the behavioral experiments. M. Nakayama contributed to the experiments on autopsied brains. Tatsunori Tanaka, Y. Ago, and H. Hashimoto contributed to the behavioral experiments for neuron-specific Smc3 heterozygous deficient mice. T. Yamashita coordinated and directed the project.

Armstrong
,
D.
,
J.K.
Dunn
,
B.
Antalffy
, and
R.
Trivedi
.
1995
.
Selective dendritic alterations in the cortex of Rett syndrome
.
J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol.
54
:
195
201
.
Armstrong
,
D.D.
,
K.
Dunn
, and
B.
Antalffy
.
1998
.
Decreased dendritic branching in frontal, motor and limbic cortex in Rett syndrome compared with trisomy 21
.
J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol.
57
:
1013
1017
.
Basile
,
E.
,
L.
Villa
,
A.
Selicorni
, and
M.
Molteni
.
2007
.
The behavioural phenotype of Cornelia de Lange Syndrome: A study of 56 individuals
.
J. Intellect. Disabil. Res.
51
:
671
681
.
Becker
,
L.E.
,
D.L.
Armstrong
, and
F.
Chan
.
1986
.
Dendritic atrophy in children with Down’s syndrome
.
Ann. Neurol.
20
:
520
526
.
Becker
,
L.
,
T.
Mito
,
S.
Takashima
, and
K.
Onodera
.
1991
.
Growth and development of the brain in Down syndrome
.
Prog. Clin. Biol. Res.
373
:
133
152
.
Berney
,
T.P.
,
M.
Ireland
, and
J.
Burn
.
1999
.
Behavioural phenotype of Cornelia de Lange syndrome
.
Arch. Dis. Child.
81
:
333
336
.
Bourgeron
,
T.
2015
.
From the genetic architecture to synaptic plasticity in autism spectrum disorder
.
Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
16
:
551
563
.
Crawley
,
J.N.
2004
.
Designing mouse behavioral tasks relevant to autistic-like behaviors
.
Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev.
10
:
248
258
.
Cruz-Martín
,
A.
,
M.
Crespo
, and
C.
Portera-Cailliau
.
2010
.
Delayed stabilization of dendritic spines in fragile X mice
.
J. Neurosci.
30
:
7793
7803
.
Donze
,
D.
,
C.R.
Adams
,
J.
Rine
, and
R.T.
Kamakaka
.
1999
.
The boundaries of the silenced HMR domain in Saccharomyces cerevisiae
.
Genes Dev.
13
:
698
708
.
Dorsett
,
D.
2007
.
Roles of the sister chromatid cohesion apparatus in gene expression, development, and human syndromes
.
Chromosoma.
116
:
1
13
.
Dulawa
,
S.C.
, and
R.
Hen
.
2005
.
Recent advances in animal models of chronic antidepressant effects: The novelty-induced hypophagia test
.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
29
:
771
783
.
Feng
,
J.
,
Y.
Zhou
,
S.L.
Campbell
,
T.
Le
,
E.
Li
,
J.D.
Sweatt
,
A.J.
Silva
, and
G.
Fan
.
2010
.
Dnmt1 and Dnmt3a maintain DNA methylation and regulate synaptic function in adult forebrain neurons
.
Nat. Neurosci.
13
:
423
430
.
Fujiwara
,
K.
,
Y.
Fujita
,
A.
Kasai
,
Y.
Onaka
,
H.
Hashimoto
,
H.
Okada
, and
T.
Yamashita
.
2016
.
Deletion of JMJD2B in neurons leads to defective spine maturation, hyperactive behavior and memory deficits in mouse
.
Transl. Psychiatry.
6
:
e766
.
Guacci
,
V.
,
D.
Koshland
, and
A.
Strunnikov
.
1997
.
A direct link between sister chromatid cohesion and chromosome condensation revealed through the analysis of MCD1 in S. cerevisiae
.
Cell.
91
:
47
57
.
Hayashi
,
M.L.
,
S.Y.
Choi
,
B.S.
Rao
,
H.Y.
Jung
,
H.K.
Lee
,
D.
Zhang
,
S.
Chattarji
,
A.
Kirkwood
, and
S.
Tonegawa
.
2004
.
Altered cortical synaptic morphology and impaired memory consolidation in forebrain-specific dominant-negative PAK transgenic mice
.
Neuron.
42
:
773
787
.
Hinton
,
V.J.
,
W.T.
Brown
,
K.
Wisniewski
, and
R.D.
Rudelli
.
1991
.
Analysis of neocortex in three males with the fragile X syndrome
.
Am. J. Med. Genet.
41
:
289
294
.
Hu
,
B.
,
N.
Petela
,
A.
Kurze
,
K.L.
Chan
,
C.
Chapard
, and
K.
Nasmyth
.
2015
.
Biological chromodynamics: A general method for measuring protein occupancy across the genome by calibrating ChIP-seq
.
Nucleic Acids Res.
43
:
e132
.
Imayoshi
,
I.
,
T.
Ohtsuka
,
D.
Metzger
,
P.
Chambon
, and
R.
Kageyama
.
2006
.
Temporal regulation of Cre recombinase activity in neural stem cells
.
Genesis.
44
:
233
238
.
Imayoshi
,
I.
,
M.
Sakamoto
,
T.
Ohtsuka
,
K.
Takao
,
T.
Miyakawa
,
M.
Yamaguchi
,
K.
Mori
,
T.
Ikeda
,
S.
Itohara
, and
R.
Kageyama
.
2008
.
Roles of continuous neurogenesis in the structural and functional integrity of the adult forebrain
.
Nat. Neurosci.
11
:
1153
1161
.
Izumi
,
K.
,
R.
Nakato
,
Z.
Zhang
,
A.C.
Edmondson
,
S.
Noon
,
M.C.
Dulik
,
R.
Rajagopalan
,
C.P.
Venditti
,
K.
Gripp
,
J.
Samanich
, et al
2015
.
Germline gain-of-function mutations in AFF4 cause a developmental syndrome functionally linking the super elongation complex and cohesin
.
Nat. Genet.
47
:
338
344
.
Kim
,
D.
,
G.
Pertea
,
C.
Trapnell
,
H.
Pimentel
,
R.
Kelley
, and
S.L.
Salzberg
.
2013
.
TopHat2: Accurate alignment of transcriptomes in the presence of insertions, deletions and gene fusions
.
Genome Biol.
14
:
R36
.
Kim
,
I.J.
,
H.N.
Beck
,
P.J.
Lein
, and
D.
Higgins
.
2002
.
Interferon gamma induces retrograde dendritic retraction and inhibits synapse formation
.
J. Neurosci.
22
:
4530
4539
.
Kimura
,
A.
,
Y.
Ishida
,
T.
Hayashi
,
T.
Wada
,
H.
Yokoyama
,
T.
Sugaya
,
N.
Mukaida
, and
T.
Kondo
.
2006
.
Interferon-gamma plays protective roles in sodium arsenite-induced renal injury by up-regulating intrarenal multidrug resistance-associated protein 1 expression
.
Am. J. Pathol.
169
:
1118
1128
.
Kohwi
,
M.
, and
C.Q.
Doe
.
2013
.
Temporal fate specification and neural progenitor competence during development
.
Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
14
:
823
838
.
Komada
,
M.
,
K.
Takao
, and
T.
Miyakawa
.
2008
.
J. Vis. Exp.
1088
.
Komata
,
M.
,
Y.
Katou
,
H.
Tanaka
,
R.
Nakato
,
K.
Shirahige
, and
M.
Bando
.
2014
.
Chromatin immunoprecipitation protocol for mammalian cells
.
Methods Mol. Biol.
1164
:
33
38
.
Krantz
,
I.D.
,
J.
McCallum
,
C.
DeScipio
,
M.
Kaur
,
L.A.
Gillis
,
D.
Yaeger
,
L.
Jukofsky
,
N.
Wasserman
,
A.
Bottani
,
C.A.
Morris
, et al
2004
.
Cornelia de Lange syndrome is caused by mutations in NIPBL, the human homolog of Drosophila melanogaster Nipped-B
.
Nat. Genet.
36
:
631
635
.
Langmead
,
B.
, and
S.L.
Salzberg
.
2012
.
Fast gapped-read alignment with Bowtie 2
.
Nat. Methods.
9
:
357
359
.
Li
,
M.
,
G.H.
Liu
, and
J.C.
Izpisua Belmonte
.
2012
.
Navigating the epigenetic landscape of pluripotent stem cells
.
Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol.
13
:
524
535
.
Liu
,
J.
,
Z.
Zhang
,
M.
Bando
,
T.
Itoh
,
M.A.
Deardorff
,
J.R.
Li
,
D.
Clark
,
M.
Kaur
,
K.
Tatsuro
,
A.D.
Kline
, et al
2010
.
Genome-wide DNA methylation analysis in cohesin mutant human cell lines
.
Nucleic Acids Res.
38
:
5657
5671
.
Losada
,
A.
,
M.
Hirano
, and
T.
Hirano
.
1998
.
Identification of Xenopus SMC protein complexes required for sister chromatid cohesion
.
Genes Dev.
12
:
1986
1997
.
Matsumura
,
H.
,
H.
Hasuwa
,
N.
Inoue
,
M.
Ikawa
, and
M.
Okabe
.
2004
.
Lineage-specific cell disruption in living mice by Cre-mediated expression of diphtheria toxin A chain
.
Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun.
321
:
275
279
.
Meshi
,
D.
,
M.R.
Drew
,
M.
Saxe
,
M.S.
Ansorge
,
D.
David
,
L.
Santarelli
,
C.
Malapani
,
H.
Moore
, and
R.
Hen
.
2006
.
Hippocampal neurogenesis is not required for behavioral effects of environmental enrichment
.
Nat. Neurosci.
9
:
729
731
.
Michaelis
,
C.
,
R.
Ciosk
, and
K.
Nasmyth
.
1997
.
Cohesins: Chromosomal proteins that prevent premature separation of sister chromatids
.
Cell.
91
:
35
45
.
Minamino
,
M.
,
M.
Ishibashi
,
R.
Nakato
,
K.
Akiyama
,
H.
Tanaka
,
Y.
Kato
,
L.
Negishi
,
T.
Hirota
,
T.
Sutani
,
M.
Bando
, and
K.
Shirahige
.
2015
.
Esco1 acetylates cohesin via a mechanism different from that of Esco2
.
Curr. Biol.
25
:
1694
1706
.
Misteli
,
T.
2007
.
Beyond the sequence: cellular organization of genome function
.
Cell.
128
:
787
800
.
Mitsui
,
S.
,
M.
Saito
,
K.
Mori
, and
Y.
Yoshihara
.
2007
.
A transcriptional enhancer that directs telencephalon-specific transgene expression in mouse brain
.
Cereb. Cortex.
17
:
522
530
.
Miyakawa
,
T.
,
M.
Yamada
,
A.
Duttaroy
, and
J.
Wess
.
2001
.
Hyperactivity and intact hippocampus-dependent learning in mice lacking the M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor
.
J. Neurosci.
21
:
5239
5250
.
Moy
,
S.S.
,
J.J.
Nadler
,
A.
Perez
,
R.P.
Barbaro
,
J.M.
Johns
,
T.R.
Magnuson
,
J.
Piven
, and
J.N.
Crawley
.
2004
.
Sociability and preference for social novelty in five inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like behavior in mice
.
Genes Brain Behav.
3
:
287
302
.
Muramatsu
,
K.
,
Y.
Hashimoto
,
T.
Uemura
,
M.
Kunii
,
R.
Harada
,
T.
Sato
,
A.
Morikawa
, and
A.
Harada
.
2008
.
Neuron-specific recombination by Cre recombinase inserted into the murine tau locus
.
Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun.
370
:
419
423
.
Musio
,
A.
,
A.
Selicorni
,
M.L.
Focarelli
,
C.
Gervasini
,
D.
Milani
,
S.
Russo
,
P.
Vezzoni
, and
L.
Larizza
.
2006
.
X-linked Cornelia de Lange syndrome owing to SMC1L1 mutations
.
Nat. Genet.
38
:
528
530
.
Nakanishi
,
M.
,
M.A.
Deardorff
,
D.
Clark
,
S.E.
Levy
,
I.
Krantz
, and
M.
Pipan
.
2012
.
Investigation of autistic features among individuals with mild to moderate Cornelia de Lange syndrome
.
Am. J. Med. Genet. A.
158A
:
1841
1847
.
Nakato
,
R.
,
T.
Itoh
, and
K.
Shirahige
.
2013
.
DROMPA: Easy-to-handle peak calling and visualization software for the computational analysis and validation of ChIP-seq data
.
Genes Cells.
18
:
589
601
.
Nasmyth
,
K.
2011
.
Cohesin: a catenase with separate entry and exit gates?
Nat. Cell Biol.
13
:
1170
1177
.
Nasmyth
,
K.
, and
C.H.
Haering
.
2005
.
The structure and function of SMC and kleisin complexes
.
Annu. Rev. Biochem.
74
:
595
648
.
Njung’e
,
K.
, and
S.L.
Handley
.
1991
.
Evaluation of marble-burying behavior as a model of anxiety
.
Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.
38
:
63
67
.
Nord
,
A.S.
,
K.
Pattabiraman
,
A.
Visel
, and
J.L.
Rubenstein
.
2015
.
Genomic perspectives of transcriptional regulation in forebrain development
.
Neuron.
85
:
27
47
.
Ohira
,
K.
,
K.
Kobayashi
,
K.
Toyama
,
H.K.
Nakamura
,
H.
Shoji
,
K.
Takao
,
R.
Takeuchi
,
S.
Yamaguchi
,
M.
Kataoka
,
S.
Otsuka
, et al
2013
.
Synaptosomal-associated protein 25 mutation induces immaturity of the dentate granule cells of adult mice
.
Mol. Brain.
6
:
12
.
Pauli
,
A.
,
F.
Althoff
,
R.A.
Oliveira
,
S.
Heidmann
,
O.
Schuldiner
,
C.F.
Lehner
,
B.J.
Dickson
, and
K.
Nasmyth
.
2008
.
Cell-type-specific TEV protease cleavage reveals cohesin functions in Drosophila neurons
.
Dev. Cell.
14
:
239
251
.
Paxinos
,
G.
, and
K.B.J.
Franklin
.
1997
.
The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates.
Academic Press
,
San Diego
. 186 pp.
Prinz
,
M.
,
B.
Prinz
, and
E.
Schulz
.
1997
.
The growth of non-pyramidal neurons in the primary motor cortex of man: a Golgi study
.
Histol. Histopathol.
12
:
895
900
.
Reimand
,
J.
,
T.
Arak
, and
J.
Vilo
.
2011
.
g:Profiler--a web server for functional interpretation of gene lists (2011 update)
.
Nucleic Acids Res.
39
:
W307
W315
.
Richards
,
C.
,
J.
Moss
,
L.
O’Farrell
,
G.
Kaur
, and
C.
Oliver
.
2009
.
Social anxiety in Cornelia de Lange syndrome
.
J. Autism Dev. Disord.
39
:
1155
1162
.
Rollins
,
R.A.
,
P.
Morcillo
, and
D.
Dorsett
.
1999
.
Nipped-B, a Drosophila homologue of chromosomal adherins, participates in activation by remote enhancers in the cut and Ultrabithorax genes
.
Genetics.
152
:
577
593
.
Rudelli
,
R.D.
,
W.T.
Brown
,
K.
Wisniewski
,
E.C.
Jenkins
,
M.
Laure-Kamionowska
,
F.
Connell
, and
H.M.
Wisniewski
.
1985
.
Adult fragile X syndrome. Clinico-neuropathologic findings
.
Acta Neuropathol.
67
:
289
295
.
Schuldiner
,
O.
,
D.
Berdnik
,
J.M.
Levy
,
J.S.
Wu
,
D.
Luginbuhl
,
A.C.
Gontang
, and
L.
Luo
.
2008
.
piggyBac-based mosaic screen identifies a postmitotic function for cohesin in regulating developmental axon pruning
.
Dev. Cell.
14
:
227
238
.
Shlyueva
,
D.
,
G.
Stampfel
, and
A.
Stark
.
2014
.
Transcriptional enhancers: From properties to genome-wide predictions
.
Nat. Rev. Genet.
15
:
272
286
.
Shoji
,
H.
,
H.
Hagihara
,
K.
Takao
,
S.
Hattori
, and
T.
Miyakawa
.
2012
.
T-maze forced alternation and left-right discrimination tasks for assessing working and reference memory in mice
.
J. Vis. Exp.
3300
.
Shoji
,
H.
,
K.
Takao
,
S.
Hattori
, and
T.
Miyakawa
.
2014
.
Contextual and cued fear conditioning test using a video analyzing system in mice
.
J. Vis. Exp.
50871
.
Sholl
,
D.A.
1953
.
Dendritic organization in the neurons of the visual and motor cortices of the cat
.
J. Anat.
87
:
387
406
.
Stasevich
,
T.J.
,
Y.
Hayashi-Takanaka
,
Y.
Sato
,
K.
Maehara
,
Y.
Ohkawa
,
K.
Sakata-Sogawa
,
M.
Tokunaga
,
T.
Nagase
,
N.
Nozaki
,
J.G.
McNally
, and
H.
Kimura
.
2014
.
Regulation of RNA polymerase II activation by histone acetylation in single living cells
.
Nature.
516
:
272
275
.
Takao
,
K.
, and
T.
Miyakawa
.
2006
.
Light/dark transition test for mice
.
J. Vis. Exp.
104
.
Takao
,
K.
,
K.
Toyama
,
K.
Nakanishi
,
S.
Hattori
,
H.
Takamura
,
M.
Takeda
,
T.
Miyakawa
, and
R.
Hashimoto
.
2008
.
Impaired long-term memory retention and working memory in sdy mutant mice with a deletion in Dtnbp1, a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia
.
Mol. Brain.
1
:
11
.
Takashima
,
S.
,
A.
Ieshima
,
H.
Nakamura
, and
L.E.
Becker
.
1989
.
Dendrites, dementia and the Down syndrome
.
Brain Dev.
11
:
131
133
.
Takizawa
,
T.
, and
E.
Meshorer
.
2008
.
Chromatin and nuclear architecture in the nervous system
.
Trends Neurosci.
31
:
343
352
.
Tanda
,
K.
,
A.
Nishi
,
N.
Matsuo
,
K.
Nakanishi
,
N.
Yamasaki
,
T.
Sugimoto
,
K.
Toyama
,
K.
Takao
, and
T.
Miyakawa
.
2009
.
Abnormal social behavior, hyperactivity, impaired remote spatial memory, and increased D1-mediated dopaminergic signaling in neuronal nitric oxide synthase knockout mice
.
Mol. Brain.
2
:
19
.
Thomas
,
A.
,
A.
Burant
,
N.
Bui
,
D.
Graham
,
L.A.
Yuva-Paylor
, and
R.
Paylor
.
2009
.
Marble burying reflects a repetitive and perseverative behavior more than novelty-induced anxiety
.
Psychopharmacology (Berl.).
204
:
361
373
.
Tonkin
,
E.T.
,
T.J.
Wang
,
S.
Lisgo
,
M.J.
Bamshad
, and
T.
Strachan
.
2004
.
NIPBL, encoding a homolog of fungal Scc2-type sister chromatid cohesion proteins and fly Nipped-B, is mutated in Cornelia de Lange syndrome
.
Nat. Genet.
36
:
636
641
.
Trapnell
,
C.
,
D.G.
Hendrickson
,
M.
Sauvageau
,
L.
Goff
,
J.L.
Rinn
, and
L.
Pachter
.
2013
.
Differential analysis of gene regulation at transcript resolution with RNA-seq
.
Nat. Biotechnol.
31
:
46
53
.
Tropea
,
D.
,
G.
Kreiman
,
A.
Lyckman
,
S.
Mukherjee
,
H.
Yu
,
S.
Horng
, and
M.
Sur
.
2006
.
Gene expression changes and molecular pathways mediating activity-dependent plasticity in visual cortex
.
Nat. Neurosci.
9
:
660
668
.
Tsujimura
,
A.
,
M.
Matsuki
,
K.
Takao
,
K.
Yamanishi
,
T.
Miyakawa
, and
T.
Hashimoto-Gotoh
.
2008
.
Mice lacking the kf-1 gene exhibit increased anxiety- but not despair-like behavior
.
Front. Behav. Neurosci.
2
:
4
.
Urdinguio
,
R.G.
,
J.V.
Sanchez-Mut
, and
M.
Esteller
.
2009
.
Epigenetic mechanisms in neurological diseases: Genes, syndromes, and therapies
.
Lancet Neurol.
8
:
1056
1072
.
Wendt
,
K.S.
,
K.
Yoshida
,
T.
Itoh
,
M.
Bando
,
B.
Koch
,
E.
Schirghuber
,
S.
Tsutsumi
,
G.
Nagae
,
K.
Ishihara
,
T.
Mishiro
, et al
2008
.
Cohesin mediates transcriptional insulation by CCCTC-binding factor
.
Nature.
451
:
796
801
.
Yao
,
I.
,
K.
Takao
,
T.
Miyakawa
,
S.
Ito
, and
M.
Setou
.
2011
.
Synaptic E3 ligase SCRAPPER in contextual fear conditioning: extensive behavioral phenotyping of Scrapper heterozygote and overexpressing mutant mice
.
PLoS One.
6
:
e17317
(published erratum appears in PLoS One. 2011. 6:6).

Abbreviations used:
CdLS

Cornelia de Lange syndrome

ChIP

chromatin IP

CNS

central nervous system

DEG

differentially expressed gene

ES

embryonic stem

GFAP

glial fibrillary acidic protein

GO

gene ontology

IP

immunoprecipitation

MBP

myelin basic protein

OR

occupancy ratio

This article is available under a Creative Commons License (Attribution 4.0 International, as described at https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

Supplementary data